Permanent
E-mail Addresses Freethought Audio
Tapes Products for Freethinkers

Library: Historical Documents: Charles Darwin: Descent of Man: Chapter 3


This file has been made available by the Bank Of Wisdom.

Descent of Man [ 1871 ]

Charles Darwin [ 1809 - 1882 ]

 

Chapter III - Comparison of the Mental Powers of Man and the Lower Animals




  WE HAVE seen in the last two chapters that man bears in his bodily

structure clear traces of his descent from some lower form; but it may

be urged that, as man differs so greatly in his mental power from

all other animals, there must be some error in this conclusion. No

doubt the difference in this respect is enormous, even if we compare

the mind of one of the lowest savages, who has no words to express any

number higher than four, and who uses hardly any abstract terms for

common objects or for the affections,* with that of the most highly

organised ape. The difference would, no doubt, still remain immense,

even if one of the higher apes had been improved or civilised as

much as a dog has been in comparison with its parent-form, the wolf or

jackal. The Fuegians rank amongst the lowest barbarians; but I was

continually struck with surprise how closely the three natives on

board H. M. S. Beagle, who had lived some years in England, and

could talk a little English, resembled us in disposition and in most

of our mental faculties. If no organic being excepting man had

possessed any mental power, or if his powers had been of a wholly

different nature from those of the lower animals, then we should never

have been able to convince ourselves that our high faculties had

been gradually developed. But it can be shewn that there is no

fundamental difference of this kind. We must also admit that there

is a much wider interval in mental power between one of the lowest

fishes, as a lamprey or lancelet, and one of the higher apes, than

between an ape and man; yet this interval is filled up by numberless

gradations.



  * See the evidence on those points, as given by Lubbock, Prehistoric

Times, p. 354, &c.



  Nor is the difference slight in moral disposition between a

barbarian, such as the man described by the old navigator Byron, who

dashed his child on the rocks for dropping a basket of sea-urchins,

and a Howard or Clarkson; and in intellect, between a savage who

uses hardly any abstract terms, and a Newton or Shakespeare.

Differences of this kind between the highest men of the highest

races and the lowest savages, are connected by the finest

gradations. Therefore it is possible that they might pass and be

developed into each other.

  My object in this chapter is to shew that there is no fundamental

difference between man and the higher mammals in their mental

faculties. Each division of the subject might have been extended

into a separate essay, but must here be treated briefly. As no

classification of the mental powers has been universally accepted, I

shall arrange my remarks in the order most convenient for my

purpose; and will select those facts which have struck me most, with

the hope that they may produce some effect on the reader.

  With respect to animals very low in the scale, I shall give some

additional facts under Sexual Selection, shewing that their mental

powers are much higher than might have been expected. The

variability of the faculties in the individuals of the same species is

an important point for us, and some few illustrations will here be

given. But it would be superfluous to enter into many details on

this head, for I have found on frequent enquiry, that it is the

unanimous opinion of all those who have long attended to animals of

many kinds, including birds, that the individuals differ greatly in

every mental characteristic. In what manner the mental powers were

first developed in the lowest organisms, is as hopeless an enquiry

as how life itself first originated. These are problems for the

distant future, if they are ever to be solved by man.

  As man possesses the same senses as the lower animals, his

fundamental intuitions must be the same. Man has also some few

instincts in common, as that of self-preservation, sexual love, the

love of the mother for her new-born offspring, the desire possessed by

the latter to suck, and so forth. But man, perhaps, has somewhat fewer

instincts than those possessed by the animals which come next to him

in the series. The orang in the Eastern islands, and the chimpanzee in

Africa, build platforms on which they sleep; and, as both species

follow the same habit, it might be argued that this was due to

instinct, but we cannot feel sure that it is not the result of both

animals having similar wants, and possessing similar powers of

reasoning. These apes, as we may assume, avoid the many poisonous

fruits of the tropics, and man has no such knowledge: but as our

domestic animals, when taken to foreign lands, and when first turned

out in the spring, often eat poisonous herbs, which they afterwards

avoid, we cannot feel sure that the apes do not learn from their own

experience or from that of their parents what fruits to select. It is,

however, certain, as we shall presently see, that apes have an

instinctive dread of serpents, and probably of other dangerous

animals.

  The fewness and the comparative simplicity of the instincts in the

higher animals are remarkable in contrast with those of the lower

animals. Cuvier maintained that instinct and intelligence stand in

an inverse ratio to each other; and some have thought that the

intellectual faculties of the higher animals have been gradually

developed from their instincts. But Pouchet, in an interesting essay,*

has shewn that no such inverse ratio really exists. Those insects

which possess the most wonderful instincts are certainly the most

intelligent. In the vertebrate series, the least intelligent

members, namely fishes and amphibians, do not possess complex

instincts; and amongst mammals the animal most remarkable for its

instincts, namely the beaver, is highly intelligent, as will be

admitted by every one who has read Mr. Morgan's excellent work.*(2)



  * "L'Instinct chez les insectes," Revue des Deux Mondes, Feb., 1870,

p. 690.

  *(2) The American Beaver and His Works, 1868.



  Although the first dawnings of intelligence, according to Mr.

Herbert Spencer,* have been developed through the multiplication and

coordination of reflex actions, and although many of the simpler

instincts graduate into reflex actions, and can hardly be

distinguished from them, as in the case of young animals sucking,

yet the more complex instincts seem to have originated independently

of intelligence. I am, however, very far from wishing to deny that

instinctive actions may lose their fixed and untaught character, and

be replaced by others performed by the aid of the free will. On the

other hand, some intelligent actions, after being performed during

several generations, become converted into instincts and are

inherited, as when birds on oceanic islands learn to avoid man.

These actions may then be said to be degraded in character, for they

are no longer performed through reason or from experience. But the

greater number of the more complex instincts appear to have been

gained in a wholly different manner, through the natural selection

of variations of simpler instinctive actions. Such variations appear

to arise from the same unknown causes acting on the cerebral

organisation, which induce slight variations or individual differences

in other parts of the body; and these variations, owing to our

ignorance, are often said to arise spontaneously. We can, I think,

come to no other conclusion with respect to the origin of the more

complex instincts, when we reflect on the marvellous instincts of

sterile worker-ants and bees, which leave no offspring to inherit

the effects of experience and of modified habits.



  * The Principles of Psychology, 2nd ed., 1870, pp. 418-443.



  Although, as we learn from the above-mentioned insects and the

beaver, a high degree of intelligence is certainly compatible with

complex instincts, and although actions, at first learnt voluntarily

can soon through habit be performed with the quickness and certainty

of a reflex action, yet it is not improbable that there is a certain

amount of interference between the development of free intelligence

and of instinct,- which latter implies some inherited modification

of the brain. Little is known about the functions of the brain, but we

can perceive that as the intellectual powers become highly

developed, the various parts of the brain must be connected by very

intricate channels of the freest intercommunication; and as a

consequence each separate part would perhaps tend to be less well

fitted to answer to particular sensations or associations in a

definite and inherited- that is instinctive- manner. There seems

even to exist some relation between a low degree of intelligence and a

strong tendency to the formation of fixed, though not inherited

habits; for as a sagacious physician remarked to me, persons who are

slightly imbecile tend to act in everything by routine or habit; and

they are rendered much happier if this is encouraged.

  I have thought this digression worth giving, because we may easily

underrate the mental powers of the higher animals, and especially of

man, when we compare their actions founded on the memory of past

events, on foresight, reason, and imagination, with exactly similar

actions instinctively performed by the lower animals; in this latter

case the capacity of performing such actions has been gained, step

by step, through the variability of the mental organs and natural

selection, without any conscious intelligence on the part of the

animal during each successive generation. No doubt, as Mr. Wallace has

argued,* much of the intelligent work done by man is due to

imitation and not to reason; but there is this great difference

between his actions and many of those performed by the lower

animals, namely, that man cannot, on his first trial, make, for

instance, a stone hatchet or a canoe, through his power of

imitation. He has to learn his work by practice; a beaver, on the

other hand, can make its dam or canal, and a bird its nest, as well,

or nearly as well, and a spider its wonderful web, quite as

well,*(2) the first time it tries as when old and experienced.



  * Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection, 1870, p. 212.

  *(2) For the evidence on this head, see Mr. J. Traherne

Moggridge's most interesting work, Harvesting Ants and Trap-Door

Spiders, 1873, pp. 126, 128.



  To return to our immediate subject: the lower animals, like man,

manifestly feel pleasure and pain, happiness and misery. Happiness

is never better exhibited than by young animals, such as puppies,

kittens, lambs, &c., when playing together, like our own children.

Even insects play together, as has been described by that excellent

observer, P. Huber,* who saw ants chasing and pretending to bite

each other, like so many puppies.



  * Recherches sur les Moeurs des Fourmis, 1810, p. 173.



  The fact that the lower animals are excited by the same emotions

as ourselves is so well established, that it will not be necessary

to weary the reader by many details. Terror acts in the same manner on

them as on us, causing the muscles to tremble, the heart to palpitate,

the sphincters to be relaxed, and the hair to stand on end. Suspicion,

the offspring of fear, is eminently characteristic of most wild

animals. It is, I think, impossible to read the account given by Sir

E. Tennent, of the behaviour of the female elephants, used as

decoys, without admitting that they intentionally practise deceit, and

well know what they are about. Courage and timidity are extremely

variable qualities in the individuals of the same species, as is

plainly seen in our dogs. Some dogs and horses are ill-tempered, and

easily turn sulky; others are good-tempered; and these qualities are

certainly inherited. Every one knows how liable animals are to furious

rage, and how plainly they shew it. Many, and probably true, anecdotes

have been published on the long-delayed and artful revenge of

various animals. The accurate Rengger, and Brehm* state that the

American and African monkeys which they kept tame, certainly

revenged themselves. Sir Andrew Smith, a zoologist whose scrupulous

accuracy was known to many persons, told me the following story of

which he was himself an eye-witness; at the Cape of Good Hope an

officer had often plagued a certain baboon, and the animal, seeing him

approaching one Sunday for parade, poured water into a hole and

hastily made some thick mud, which he skilfully dashed over the

officer as he passed by, to the amusement of many bystanders. For long

afterwards the baboon rejoiced and triumphed whenever he saw his

victim.



  * All the following statements, given on the authority of these

two naturalists, are taken from Rengger's Naturgesch. der

Saugethiere von Paraguay, 1830, ss. 41-57, and from Brehm's

Thierleben, B. i., ss. 10-87.



  The love of a dog for his master is notorious; as an old writer

quaintly says,* "A dog is the only thing on this earth that luvs you

more than he luvs himself."



  * Quoted by Dr. Lauder Lindsay, in his "Physiology of Mind in the

Lower Animals," Journal of Mental Science, April, 1871, p. 38.



  In the agony of death a dog has been known to caress his master, and

every one has heard of the dog suffering under vivisection, who licked

the hand of the operator; this man, unless the operation was fully

justified by an increase of our knowledge, or unless he had a heart of

stone, must have felt remorse to the last hour of his life.

  As Whewell* has well asked, "Who that reads the touching instances

of maternal affection, related so often of the women of all nations,

and of the females of all animals, can doubt that the principle of

action is the same in the two cases?" We see maternal affection

exhibited in the most trifling details; thus Rengger observed an

American monkey (a Cebus) carefully driving away the flies which

plagued her infant; and Duvaucel saw a Hylobates washing the faces

of her young ones in a stream. So intense is the grief of female

monkeys for the loss of their young, that it invariably caused the

death of certain kinds kept under confinement by Brehm in N. Africa.

Orphan monkeys were always adopted and carefully guarded by the

other monkeys, both males and females. One female baboon had so

capacious a heart that she not only adopted young monkeys of other

species, but stole young dogs and cats, which she continually

carried about. Her kindness, however, did not go so far as to share

her food with her adopted offspring, at which Brehm was surprised,

as his monkeys always divided everything quite fairly with their own

young ones. An adopted kitten scratched this affectionate baboon,

who certainly had a fine intellect, for she was much astonished at

being scratched, and immediately examined the kitten's feet, and

without more ado bit off the claws.*(2) In the Zoological Gardens, I

heard from the keeper that an old baboon (C. chacma) had adopted a

Rhesus monkey; but when a young drill and mandrill were placed in

the cage, she seemed to perceive that these monkeys, though distinct

species, were her nearer relatives, for she at once rejected the

Rhesus and adopted both of them. The young Rhesus, as I saw, was

greatly discontented at being thus rejected, and it would, like a

naughty child, annoy and attack the young drill and mandrill

whenever it could do so with safety; this conduct exciting great

indignation in the old baboon. Monkeys will also, according to

Brehm, defend their master when attacked by any one, as well as dogs

to whom they are attached, from the attacks of other dogs. But we here

trench on the subjects of sympathy and fidelity, to which I shall

recur. Some of Brehm's monkeys took much delight in teasing a

certain old dog whom they disliked, as well as other animals, in

various ingenious ways.



  * Bridgewater Treatise, p. 263.

  *(2) A critic, without any grounds (Quarterly Review, July, 1871, p.

72), disputes the possibility of this act as described by Brehm, for

the sake of discrediting my work. Therefore I tried, and found that

I could readily seize with my own teeth the sharp little claws of a

kitten nearly five weeks old.



  Most of the more complex emotions are common to the higher animals

and ourselves. Every one has seen how jealous a dog is of his master's

affection, if lavished on any other creature; and I have observed

the same fact with monkeys. This shews that animals not only love, but

have desire to be loved. Animals manifestly feel emulation. They

love approbation or praise; and a dog carrying a basket for his master

exhibits in a high degree self-complacency or pride. There can, I

think, be no doubt that a dog feels shame, as distinct from fear,

and something very like modesty when begging too often for food. A

great dog scorns the snarling of a little dog, and this may be

called magnanimity. Several observers have stated that monkeys

certainly dislike being laughed at; and they sometimes invent

imaginary offences. In the Zoological Gardens I saw a baboon who

always got into a furious rage when his keeper took out a letter or

book and read it aloud to him; and his rage was so violent that, as

I witnessed on one occasion, he bit his own leg till the blood flowed.

Dogs shew what may be fairly called a sense of humour, as distinct

from mere play; if a bit of stick or other such object be thrown to

one, he will often carry it away for a short distance; and then

squatting down with it on the ground close before him, will wait until

his master comes quite close to take it away. The dog will then

seize it and rush away in triumph, repeating the same manoeuvre, and

evidently enjoying the practical joke.

  We will now turn to the more intellectual emotions and faculties,

which are very important, as forming the basis for the development

of the higher mental powers. Animals manifestly enjoy excitement,

and suffer from ennui, as may be seen with dogs, and, according to

Rengger, with monkeys. All animals feel Wonder, and many exhibit

Curiosity. They sometimes suffer from this latter quality, as when the

hunter plays antics and thus attracts them; I have witnessed this with

deer, and so it is with the wary chamois, and with some kinds of

wild-ducks. Brehm gives a curious account of the instinctive dread,

which his monkeys exhibited, for snakes; but their curiosity was so

great that they could not desist from occasionally satiating their

horror in a most human fashion, by lifting up the lid of the box in

which the snakes were kept. I was so much surprised at this account,

that I took a stuffed and coiled-up snake into the monkey-house at the

Zoological Gardens, and the excitement thus caused was one of the most

curious spectacles which I ever beheld. Three species of Cercopithecus

were the most alarmed; they dashed about their cages, and uttered

sharp signal cries of danger, which were understood by the other

monkeys. A few young monkeys and one old Anubis baboon alone took no

notice of the snake. I then placed the stuffed specimen on the

ground in one of the larger compartments. After a time all the monkeys

collected round it in a large circle, and staring intently,

presented a most ludicrous appearance. They became extremely

nervous; so that when a wooden ball, with which they were familiar

as a plaything, was accidentally moved in the straw, under which it

was partly hidden, they all instantly started away. These monkeys

behaved very differently when a dead fish, a mouse,* a living

turtle, and other new objects were placed in their cages; for though

at first frightened, they soon approached, handled and examined

them. I then placed a live snake in a paper bag, with the mouth

loosely closed, in one of the larger compartments. One of the

monkeys immediately approached, cautiously opened the bag a little,

peeped in, and instantly dashed away. Then I witnessed what Brehm

has described, for monkey after monkey, with head raised high and

turned on one side, could not resist taking a momentary peep into

the upright bag, at the dreadful object lying quietly at the bottom.

It would almost appear as if monkeys had some notion of zoological

affinities, for those kept by Brehm exhibited a strange, though

mistaken, instinctive dread of innocent lizards and frogs. An orang,

also, has been known to be much alarmed at the first sight of a

turtle.*(2)



  * I have given a short account of their behaviour on this occasion

in my Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, p. 43.

  *(2) W. C. L. Martin, Natural History of Mammalia, 1841, p. 405.



  The principle of Imitation is strong in man, and especially, as I

have myself observed, with savages. In certain morbid states of the

brain this tendency is exaggerated to an extraordinary degree: some

hemiplegic patients and others, at the commencement of inflammatory

softening of the brain, unconsciously imitate every word which is

uttered, whether in their own or in a foreign language, and every

gesture or action which is performed near them.* Desor*(2) has

remarked that no animal voluntarily imitates an action performed by

man, until in the ascending scale we come to monkeys, which are well

known to be ridiculous mockers. Animals, however, sometimes imitate

each other's actions: thus two species of wolves, which had been

reared by dogs, learned to bark, as does sometimes the jackal,*(3) but

whether this can be called voluntary imitation is another question.

Birds imitate the songs of their parents, and sometimes of other

birds; and parrots are notorious imitators of any sound which they

often hear. Dureau de la Malle gives an account*(4) of a dog reared by

a cat, who learnt to imitate the well-known action of a cat licking

her paws, and thus washing her ears and face; this was also

witnessed by the celebrated naturalist Audouin. I have received

several confirmatory accounts; in one of these, a dog had not been

suckled by a cat, but had been brought up with one, together with

kittens, and had thus acquired the above habit, which he ever

afterwards practised during his life of thirteen years. Dureau de la

Malle's dog likewise learnt from the kittens to play with a ball by

rolling it about with his fore paws, and springing on it. A

correspondent assures me that a cat in his house used to put her

paws into jugs of milk having too narrow a mouth for her head. A

kitten of this cat soon learned the same trick, and practised it

ever afterwards, whenever there was an opportunity.



  * Dr. Bateman, On Aphasia, 1870, p. 110.

  *(2) Quoted by Vogt, Memoire sur les Microcephales, 1867, p. 168.

  *(3) The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol.

i., p. 27.

  *(4) Annales des Sciences Nat., (1st series), tom, xxii., p. 397.



  The parents of many animals, trusting to the principle of

imitation in their young, and more especially to their instinctive

or inherited tendencies, may be said to educate them. We see this when

a cat brings a live mouse to her kittens; and Dureau de la Malle has

given a curious account (in the paper above quoted) of his

observations on hawks which taught their young dexterity, as well as

judgment of distances, by first dropping through the air dead mice and

sparrows, which the young generally failed to catch, and then bringing

them live birds and letting them loose.

  Hardly any faculty is more important for the intellectual progress

of man than Attention. Animals clearly manifest this power, as when

a cat watches by a hole and prepares to spring on its prey. Wild

animals sometimes become so absorbed when thus engaged, that they

may be easily approached. Mr. Bartlett has given me a curious proof

how variable this faculty is in monkeys. A man who trains monkeys to

act in plays, used to purchase common kinds from the Zoological

Society at the price of five pounds for each; but he offered to give

double the price, if he might keep three or four of them for a few

days, in order to select one. When asked how he could possibly learn

so soon, whether a particular monkey would turn out a good actor, he

answered that it all depended on their power of attention. If when

he was talking and explaining anything to a monkey, its attention

was easily distracted, as by a fly on the wall or other trifling

object, the case was hopeless. If he tried by punishment to make an

inattentive monkey act, it turned sulky. On the other hand, a monkey

which carefully attended to him could always be trained.

  It is almost superfluous to state that animals have excellent

Memories for persons and places. A baboon at the Cape of Good Hope, as

I have been informed by Sir Andrew Smith, recognised him with joy

after an absence of nine months. I had a dog who was savage and averse

to all strangers, and I purposely tried his memory after an absence of

five years and two days. I went near the stable where he lived, and

shouted to him in my old manner; he shewed no joy, but instantly

followed me out walking, and obeyed me, exactly as if I had parted

with him only half an hour before. A train of old associations,

dormant during five years, had thus been instantaneously awakened in

his mind. Even ants, as P. Huber* has clearly shewn, recognised

their fellow-ants belonging to the same community after a separation

of four months. Animals can certainly by some means judge of the

intervals of time between recurrent events.



  * Les Moeurs des Fourmis, 1810, p. 150.



  The Imagination is one of the highest prerogatives of man. By this

faculty he unites former images and ideas, independently of the

will, and thus creates brilliant and novel results. A poet, as Jean

Paul Richter remarks,* "who must reflect whether he shall make a

character say yes or no- to the devil with him; he is only a stupid

corpse." Dreaming gives us the best notion of this power; as Jean Paul

again says, "The dream is an involuntary art of poetry." The value

of the products of our imagination depends of course on the number,

accuracy, and clearness of our impressions, on our judgment and

taste in selecting or rejecting the involuntary combinations, and to a

certain extent on our power of voluntarily combining them. As dogs,

cats, horses, and probably all the higher animals, even birds*(2) have

vivid dreams, and this is shewn by their movements and the sounds

uttered, we must admit that they possess some power of imagination.

There must be something special, which causes dogs to howl in the

night, and especially during moonlight, in that remarkable and

melancholy manner called baying. All dogs do not do so; and, according

to Houzeau,*(3) they do not then look at the moon, but at some fixed

point near the horizon. Houzeau thinks that their imaginations are

disturbed by the vague outlines of the surrounding objects, and

conjure up before them fantastic images: if this be so, their feelings

may almost be called superstitious.



  * Quoted in Dr. Maudsley's Physiology and Pathology of Mind, 1868,

pp. 19, 220.

  *(2) Dr. Jerdon, Birds of India, vol. i., 1862, p. xxi. Houzeau says

that his parakeets and canary-birds dreamt: Etudes sur les Facultes

Mentales des Animaux, tom. ii., p. 136.

  *(3) ibid., 1872, tom. ii., p. 181.



  Of all the faculties of the human mind, it will, I presume, be

admitted that Reason stands at the summit. Only a few persons now

dispute that animals possess some power of reasoning. Animals may

constantly be seen to pause, deliberate, and resolve. It is a

significant fact, that the more the habits of any particular animal

are studied by a naturalist, the more he attributes to reason and

the less to unlearnt instincts.* In future chapters we shall see

that some animals extremely low in the scale apparently display a

certain amount of reason. No doubt it is often difficult to

distinguish between the power of reason and that of instinct. For

instance. Dr. Hayes, in his work on The Open Polar Sea, repeatedly

remarks that his dogs, instead of continuing to draw the sledges in

a compact body, diverged and separated when they came to thin ice,

so that their weight might be more evenly distributed. This was

often the first warning which the travellers received that the ice was

becoming thin and dangerous. Now, did the dogs act thus from the

experience of each individual, or from the example of the older and

wiser dogs, or from an inherited habit, that is from instinct? This

instinct, may possibly have arisen since the time, long ago, when dogs

were first employed by the natives in drawing their sledges; or the

arctic wolves, the parent-stock of the Esquimaux dog, may have

acquired an instinct impelling them not to attack their prey in a

close pack, when on thin ice.



  * Mr. L. H. Morgan's work on The American Beaver, 1868, offers a

good illustration of this remark. I cannot help thinking, however,

that he goes too far in undertaking the power of instinct.



  We can only judge by the circumstances under which actions are

performed, whether they are due to instinct, or to reason, or to the

mere association of ideas: this latter principle, however, is

intimately connected with reason. A curious case has been given by

Prof. Mobius,* of a pike, separated by a plate of glass from an

adjoining aquarium stocked with fish, and who often dashed himself

with such violence against the glass in trying to catch the other

fishes, that he was sometimes completely stunned. The pike went on

thus for three months, but at last learnt caution, and ceased to do

so. The plate of glass was then removed, but the pike would not attack

these particular fishes, though he would devour others which were

afterwards introduced; so strongly was the idea of a violent shock

associated in his feeble mind with the attempt on his former

neighbours. If a savage, who had never seen a large plate-glass

window, were to dash himself even once against it, he would for a long

time afterwards associate a shock with a window-frame; but very

differently from the pike, he would probably reflect on the nature

of the impediment, and be cautious under analogous circumstances.

Now with monkeys, as we shall presently see, a painful or merely a

disagreeable impression, from an action once performed, is sometimes

sufficient to prevent the animal from repeating it. If we attribute

this difference between the monkey and the pike solely to the

association of ideas being so much stronger and more persistent in the

one than the other, though the pike often received much the more

severe injury, can we maintain in the case of man that a similar

difference implies the possession of a fundamentally different mind?



  * Die Bewegungen der Thiere, &c., 1873, p. 11.



  Houzeau relates* that, whilst crossing a wide and arid plain in

Texas, his two dogs suffered greatly from thirst, and that between

thirty and forty times they rushed down the hollows to search for

water. These hollows were not valleys, and there were no trees in

them, or any other difference in the vegetation, and as they were

absolutely dry there could have been no smell of damp earth. The

dogs behaved as if they knew that a dip in the ground offered them the

best chance of finding water, and Houzeau has often witnessed the same

behaviour in other animals.



  * Etudes sur les Facultes Mentales des Animaux, 1872, tom. ii., p.

265.



  I have seen, as I daresay have others, that when a small object is

thrown on the ground beyond the reach of one of the elephants in the

Zoological Gardens, he blows through his trunk on the ground beyond

the object, so that the current reflected on all sides may drive the

object within his reach. Again a well-known ethnologist, Mr. Westropp,

informs me that he observed in Vienna a bear deliberately making

with his paw a current in some water, which was close to the bars of

his cage, so as to draw a piece of floating bread within his reach.

These actions of the elephant and bear can hardly be attributed to

ins7tinct or inherited habit, as they would be of little use to an

animal in a state of nature. Now, what is the difference between

such actions, when performed by an uncultivated man, and by one of the

higher animals?

  The savage and the dog have often found water at a low level, and

the coincidence under such circumstances has become associated in

their minds. A cultivated man would perhaps make some general

proposition on the subject; but from all that we know of savages it is

extremely doubtful whether they would do so, and a dog certainly would

not. But a savage, as well as a dog, would search in the same way,

though frequently disappointed; and in both it seems to be equally

an act of reason, whether or not any general proposition on the

subject is consciously placed before the mind.* The same would apply

to the elephant and the bear making currents in the air or water.

The savage would certainly neither know nor care by what law the

desired movements were effected; yet his act would be guided by a rude

process of reasoning, as surely as would a philosopher in his

longest chain of deductions. There would no doubt be this difference

between him and one of the higher animals, that he would take notice

of much slighter circumstances and conditions, and would observe any

connection between them after much less experience, and this would

be of paramount importance. I kept a daily record of the actions of

one of my infants, and when he was about eleven months old, and before

he could speak a single word, I was continually struck with the

greater quickness, with which all sorts of objects and sounds were

associated together in his mind, compared with that of the most

intelligent dogs I ever knew. But the higher animals differ in exactly

the same way in this power of association from those low in the scale,

such as the pike, as well as in that of drawing inferences and of

observation.



  * Prof. Huxley has analysed with admirable clearness the mental

steps by which a man, as well as a dog, arrives at a conclusion in a

case analogous to that given in my text. See his article, "Mr.

Darwin's Critics," in the Contemporary Review, Nov., 1871, p. 462, and

in his Critiques and Essays, 1873, p. 279.



  The promptings of reason, after very short experience, are well

shewn by the following actions of American monkeys, which stand low in

their order. Rengger, a most careful observer, states that when he

first gave eggs to his monkeys in Paraguay, they smashed them, and

thus lost much of their contents; afterwards they gently hit one end

against some hard body, and picked off the bits of shell with their

fingers. After cutting themselves only once with any sharp tool,

they would not touch it again, or would handle it with the greatest

caution. Lumps of sugar were often given them wrapped up in paper; and

Rengger sometimes put a live wasp in the paper, so that in hastily

unfolding it they got stung; after this had once happened, they always

first held the packet to their ears to detect any movement within.*



  * Mr. Belt, in his most interesting work, The Naturalist in

Nicaragua, 1874, p. 119, likewise describes various actions of a tamed

Cebus, which, I think, clearly shew that this animal possessed some

reasoning power.



  The following cases relate to dogs. Mr. Colquhoun* winged two

wild-ducks, which fell on the further side of a stream; his

retriever tried to bring over both at once, but could not succeed; she

then, though never before known to ruffle a feather, deliberately

killed one, brought over the other, and returned for the dead bird.

Col. Hutchinson relates that two partridges were shot at once, one

being killed, the other wounded; the latter ran away, and was caught

by the retriever, who on her return came across the dead bird; "she

stopped, evidently greatly puzzled, and after one or two trials,

finding she could not take it up without permitting the escape of

the winged bird, she considered a moment, then deliberately murdered

it by giving it a severe crunch, and afterwards brought away both

together. This was the only known instance of her ever having wilfully

injured any game." Here we have reason though not quite perfect, for

the retriever might have brought the wounded bird first and then

returned for the dead one, as in the case of the two wild-ducks. I

give the above cases, as resting on the evidence of two independent

witnesses, and because in both instances the retrievers, after

deliberation, broke through a habit which is inherited by them (that

of not killing the game retrieved), and because they shew how strong

their reasoning faculty must have been to overcome a fixed habit.



  * The Moor and the Loch, p. 45. Col. Hutchinson on Dog Breaking,

1850, p. 46.



  I will conclude by quoting a remark by the illustrious Humboldt.*

"The muleteers in S. America say, 'I will not give you the mule

whose step is easiest, but la mas racional,- the one that reasons

best'"; and; as, he adds, "this popular expression, dictated by long

experience, combats the system of animated machines, better perhaps

than all the arguments of speculative philosophy." Nevertheless some

writers even yet deny that the higher animals possess a trace of

reason; and they endeavor to explain away, by what appears to be

mere verbiage,*(2) all such facts as those above given.



  * Personal Narrative, Eng. translat., vol. iii., p. 106.

  *(2) I am glad to find that so acute a reasoner as Mr. Leslie

Stephen ("Darwinism and Divinity," Essays on Free Thinking, 1873, p.

80), in speaking of the supposed impassable barrier between the

minds of man and the lower animals, says, "The distinctions, indeed,

which have been drawn, seem to us to rest upon no better foundation

than a great many other metaphysical distinctions; that is, the

assumption that because you can give two things different names,

they must therefore have different natures. It is difficult to

understand how anybody who has ever kept a dog, or seen an elephant,

can have any doubt as to an animal's power of performing the essential

processes of reasoning."



  It has, I think, now been shewn that man and the higher animals,

especially the primates, have some few instincts in common. All have

the same senses, intuitions, and sensations,- similar passions,

affections, and emotions, even the more complex ones, such as

jealousy, suspicion, emulation, gratitude, and magnanimity; they

practise deceit and are revengeful; they are sometimes susceptible

to ridicule, and even have a sense of humour; they feel wonder and

curiosity; they possess the same faculties of imitation, attention,

deliberation, choice, memory, imagination, the association of ideas,

and reason, though in very different degrees. The individuals of the

same species graduate in intellect from absolute imbecility to high

excellence. They are also liable to insanity, though far less often

than in the case of man.* Nevertheless, many authors have insisted

that man is divided by an insuperable barrier from all the lower

animals in in his mental faculties. I formerly made a collection of

above a score of such aphorisms, but they are almost worthless, as

their wide difference and number prove the difficulty, if not the

impossibility, of the attempt. It has been asserted that man alone

is capable of progressive improvement; that he alone makes use of

tools or fire, domesticates other animals, or possesses property; that

no animal has the power of abstraction, or of forming general

concepts, is self-conscious and comprehends itself; that no animal

employs language; that man alone has a sense of beauty, is liable to

caprice, has the feeling of gratitude, mystery, &c.; believes in

God, or is endowed with a conscience. I will hazard a few remarks on

the more important and interesting of these points.



  * See "Madness in Animals," by Dr. W. Lauder Lindsay, in Journal

of Mental Science, July, 1871.



  Archbishop Sumner formerly maintained* that man alone is capable

of progressive improvement. That he is capable of incomparably greater

and more rapid improvement than is any other animal, admits of no

dispute; and this is mainly due to his power of speaking and handing

down his acquired knowledge. With animals, looking first to the

individual, every one who has had any experience in setting traps,

knows that young animals can he caught much more easily than old ones;

and they can be much more easily approached by an enemy. Even with

respect to old animals, it is impossible to catch many in the same

place and in the same kind of trap, or to destroy them by the same

kind of poison; yet it is improbable that all should have partaken

of the poison, and impossible that all should have been caught in a

trap. They must learn caution by seeing their brethren caught or

poisoned. In North America, where the fur-bearing animals have long

been pursued, they exhibit, according to the unanimous testimony of

all observers, an almost incredible amount of sagacity, caution and

cunning; but trapping has been there so long carried on, that

inheritance may possibly have come into play. I have received

several accounts that when telegraphs are first set up in any

district, many birds kill themselves by flying against the wires,

but that in the course of a very few years they learn to avoid this

danger, by seeing, as it would appear, their comrades killed.*(2)



  * Quoted by Sir C. Lyell, Antiquity of Man, p. 497.

  *(2) For additional evidence, with details, see M. Houzeau, Etudes

sur les Facultes Mentales des Animaux, tom. ii., 1872, p. 147.



  If we look to successive generations, or to the race, there is no

doubt that birds and other animals gradually both acquire and lose

caution in relation to man or other enemies;* and this caution is

certainly in chief part an inherited habit or instinct, but in part

the result of individual experience. A good observer, Leroy,*(2)

states, that in districts where foxes are much hunted, the young, on

first leaving their burrows, are incontestably much more wary than the

old ones in districts where they are not much disturbed.



  * See, with respect to birds on oceanic islands, my Journal of

Researches during the Voyage of the "Beagle," 1845, p. 398. Also,

Origin of Species.(OOS)

  *(2) Lettres Phil. sur l'Intelligence des Animaux, nouvelle edit.,

1802, p. 86.



  Our domestic dogs are descended from wolves and jackals,* and though

they may not have gained in cunning, and may have lost in wariness and

suspicion, yet they have progressed in certain moral qualities, such

as in affection, trust-worthiness, temper, and probably in general

intelligence. The common rat has conquered and beaten several other

species throughout Europe, in parts of North America, New Zealand, and

recently in Formosa, as well as on the mainland of China. Mr.

Swinhoe,*(2) who describes these two latter cases, attributes the

victory of the common rat over the large Mus coninga to its superior

cunning; and this latter quality may probably be attributed to the

habitual exercise of all its faculties in avoiding extirpation by man,

as well as to nearly all the less cunning or weak-minded rats having

been continuously destroyed by him. It is, however, possible that

the success of the common rat may be due to its having possessed

greater cunning than its fellow-species, before it became associated

with man. To maintain, independently of any direct evidence, that no

animal during the course of ages has progressed in intellect or

other mental faculties, is to beg the question of the evolution of

species. We have seen that, according to Lartet, existing mammals

belonging to several orders have larger brains than their ancient

tertiary prototypes.



  * See the evidence on this head in chap. i., vol. i., On the

Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication.

  *(2) Proceedings Zoological Society, 1864, p. 186.



  It has often been said that no animal uses any tool; but the

chimpanzee in a state of nature cracks a native fruit, somewhat like a

walnut, with a stone.* Rengger*(2) easily taught an American monkey

thus to break open hard palm-nuts; and afterwards of its own accord,

it used stones to open other kinds of nuts, as well as boxes. It

thus also removed the soft rind of fruit that had a disagreeable

flavour. Another monkey was taught to open the lid of a large box with

a stick, and afterwards it used the stick as a lever to move heavy

bodies; and I have myself seen a young orang put a stick into a

crevice, slip his hand to the other end, and use it in the proper

manner as a lever. The tamed elephants in India are well known to

break off branches of trees and use them to drive away the flies;

and this same act has been observed in an elephant in a state of

nature.*(3) I have seen a young orang, when she thought she was

going to be whipped, cover and protect herself with a blanket or

straw. In these several cases stones and sticks were employed as

implements; but they are likewise used as weapons. Brehm*(4) states,

on the authority of the well-known traveller Schimper, that in

Abyssinia when the baboons belonging to one species (C. gelada)

descend in troops from the mountains to plunder the fields, they

sometimes encounter troops of another species (C. hamadryas), and then

a fight ensues. The Geladas roll down great stones, which the

Hamadryas try to avoid, and then both species, making a great

uproar, rush furiously against each other. Brehm, when accompanying

the Duke of Coburg-Gotha, aided in an attack with fire-arms on a troop

of baboons in the pass of Mensa in Abyssinia. The baboons in return

rolled so many stones down the mountain, some as large as a man's

head, that the attackers had to beat a hasty retreat; and the pass was

actually closed for a time against the caravan. It deserves notice

that these baboons thus acted in concert. Mr. Wallace*(5) on three

occasions saw female orangs, accompanied by their young, "breaking off

branches and the great spiny fruit of the Durian tree, with every

appearance of rage; causing such a shower of missiles as effectually

kept us from approaching too near the tree." As I have repeatedly

seen, a chimpanzee will throw any object at hand at a person who

offends him; and the before-mentioned baboon at the Cape of Good

Hope prepared mud for the purpose.



  * Savage and Wyman in Boston Journal of Natural History, vol. iv.,

1843-44, p. 383.

  *(2) Saugethiere von Paraguay, 1830, ss. 51-56.

  *(3) The Indian Field, March 4, 1871.

  *(4) Illustriertes Thierleben, B. i., s. 79, 82.

  *(5) The Malay Archipelago, vol. i., 1869, p. 87.



  In the Zoological Gardens, a monkey, which had weak teeth, used to

break open nuts with a stone; and I was assured by the keepers that

after using the stone, he hid it in the straw, and would not let any

other monkey touch it. Here, then, we have the idea of property; but

this idea is common to every dog with a bone, and to most or all birds

with their nests.

  The Duke of Argyll* remarks, that the fashioning of an implement for

a special purpose is absolutely peculiar to man; and he considers that

this forms an immeasurable gulf between him and the brutes. This is no

doubt a very important distinction; but there appears to me much truth

in Sir J. Lubbock's suggestion,*(2) that when primeval man first

used flint-stones for any purpose, he would have accidentally

splintered them, and would then have used the sharp fragments. From

this step it would be a small one to break the flints on purpose,

and not a very wide step to fashion them rudely. This latter

advance, however, may have taken long ages, if we may judge by the

immense interval of time which elapsed before the men of the neolithic

period took to grinding and polishing their stone tools. In breaking

the flints, as Sir J. Lubbock likewise remarks, sparks would have been

emitted, and in grinding them heat would have been evolved: thus the

two usual methods of "obtaining fire may have originated." The

nature of fire would have been known in the many volcanic regions

where lava occasionally flows through forests. The anthropomorphous

apes, guided probably by instinct, build for themselves temporary

platforms; but as many instincts are largely controlled by reason, the

simpler ones, such as this of building a platform, might readily

pass into a voluntary and conscious act. The orang is known to cover

itself at night with the leaves of the pandanus; and Brehm states that

one of his baboons used to protect itself from the heat of the sun

by throwing a straw-mat over its head. In these several habits, we

probably see the first steps towards some of the simpler arts, such as

rude architecture and dress, as they arose amongst the early

progenitors of man.



  * Primeval Man, 1869, pp. 145, 147.

  *(2) Prehistoric Times, 1865, p. 473, &c.



  Abstraction, General Conceptions, Self-consciousness, Mental

Individuality.- It would be very difficult for any one with even

much more knowledge than I possess, to determine how far animals

exhibit any traces of these high mental powers. This difficulty arises

from the impossibility of judging what passes through the mind of an

animal; and again, the fact that writers differ to a great extent in

the meaning which they attribute to the above terms, causes a

further difficulty. If one may judge from various articles which

have been published lately, the greatest stress seems to be laid on

the supposed entire absence in animals of the power of abstraction, or

of forming general concepts. But when a dog sees another dog at a

distance, it is often clear that he perceives that it is a dog in

the abstract; for when he gets nearer his whole manner suddenly

changes if the other dog be a friend. A recent writer remarks, that in

all such cases it is a pure assumption to assert that the mental act

is not essentially of the same nature in the animal as in man. If

either refers what he perceives with his senses to a mental concept,

then so do both.* When I say to my terrier, in an eager voice (and I

have made the trial many times), "Hi, hi, where is it?" she at once

takes it as a sign that something is to be hunted, and generally first

looks quickly all around, and then rushes into the nearest thicket, to

scent for any game, but finding nothing, she looks up into any

neighbouring tree for a squirrel. Now do not these actions clearly

shew that she had in her mind a general idea or concept that some

animal is to be discovered and hunted?



  * Mr. Hookham, in a letter to Prof. Max Muller, in the Birmingham

News, May, 1873.



  It may be freely admitted that no animal is self-conscious, if by

this term it is implied, that he reflects on such points, as whence he

comes or whither he will go, or what is life and death, and so

forth. But how can we feel sure that an old dog with an excellent

memory and some power of imagination, as shewn by his dreams, never

reflects on his past pleasures or pains in the chase? And this would

be a form of self-consciousness. On the other hand, as Buchner* has

remarked, how little can the hard worked wife of a degraded Australian

savage, who uses very few abstract words, and cannot count above four,

exert her self-consciousness, or reflect on the nature of her own

existence. It is generally admitted, that the higher animals possess

memory, attention, association, and even some imagination and

reason. If these powers, which differ much in different animals, are

capable of improvement, there seems no great improbability in more

complex faculties, such as the higher forms of abstraction, and

self-consciousness, &c., having been evolved through the development

and combination of the simpler ones. It has been urged against the

views here maintained that it is impossible to say at what point in

the ascending scale animals become capable of abstraction, &c.; but

who can say at what age this occurs in our young children? We see at

least that such powers are developed in children by imperceptible

degrees.



  * Conferences sur la Theorie Darwinienne, French translat., 1869, p.

132.



  That animals retain their mental individuality is unquestionable.

When my voice awakened a train of old associations in the mind of

the before-mentioned dog, he must have retained his mental

individuality, although every atom of his brain had probably undergone

change more than once during the interval of five years. This dog

might have brought forward the argument lately advanced to crush all

evolutionists, and said, "I abide amid all mental moods and all

material changes.... The teaching that atoms leave their impressions

as legacies to other atoms falling into the places they have vacated

is contradictory of the utterance of consciousness, and is therefore

false; but it is the teaching necessitated by evolutionism,

consequently the hypothesis is a false one."*



  * The Rev. Dr. J. M'Cann, Anti-Darwinism, 1869, p. 13.



  Language.- This faculty has justly been considered as one of the

chief distinctions between man and the lower animals. But man, as a

highly competent judge, Archbishop Whately remarks, "is not the only

animal that can make use of language to express what is passing in his

mind, and can understand, more or less, what is so expressed by

another."* In Paraguay the Cebus azarae when excited utters at least

six distinct sounds, which excite in other monkeys similar

emotions.*(2) The movements of the features and gestures of monkeys

are understood by us, and they partly understand ours, as Rengger

and others declare. It is a more remarkable fact that the dog, since

being domesticated, has learnt to bark*(3) in at least four or five

distinct tones. Although barking is a new art, no doubt the wild

parent-species of the dog expressed their feelings by cries of various

kinds. With the domesticated dog we have the bark of eagerness, as

in the chase; that of anger, as well as growling; the yelp or howl

of despair, as when shut up; the baying at night; the bark of joy,

as when starting on a walk with his master; and the very distinct

one of demand or supplication, as when wishing for a door or window to

be opened. According to Houzeau, who paid particular attention to

the subject, the domestic fowl utters at least a dozen significant

sounds.*(4)



  * Quoted in Anthropological Review, 1864, p. 158.

  *(2) Rengger, ibid., s. 45.

  *(3) See my Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,

vol. i., p. 27.

  *(4) Facultes Mentales des Animaux, tom. ii., 1872, p. 346-349.



  The habitual use of articulate language is, however, peculiar to

man; but he uses, in common with the lower animals, inarticulate cries

to express his meaning, aided by gestures and the movements of the

muscles of the face.* This especially holds good with the more

simple and vivid feelings, which are but little connected with our

higher intelligence. Our cries of pain, fear, surprise, anger,

together with their appropriate actions, and the murmur of a mother to

her beloved child are more expressive than any words. That which

distinguishes man from the lower animals is not the understanding of

articulate sounds, for, as every one knows, dogs understand many words

and sentences. In this respect they are at the same stage of

development as infants, between the ages of ten and twelve months, who

understand many words and short sentences, but cannot yet utter a

single word. It is not the mere articulation which is our

distinguishing character, for parrots and other birds possess this

power. Nor is it the mere capacity of connecting definite sounds

with definite ideas; for it is certain that some parrots, which have

been taught to speak, connect unerringly words with things, and

persons with events.*(2) The lower animals differ from man solely in

his almost infinitely larger power of associating together the most

diversified sounds and ideas; and this obviously depends on the high

development of his mental powers.



  * See a discussion on this subject in Mr. E. B. Tylor's very

interesting work, Researches into the Early History of Mankind,

1865, chaps. ii. to iv.

  *(2) I have received several detailed accounts to this effect.

Admiral Sir. B. J. Sulivan, whom I know to be a careful observer,

assures me that an African parrot, long kept in his father's house,

invariably called certain persons of the household, as well as

visitors, by their names. He said "good morning" to every one at

breakfast, and "good night" to each as they left the room at night,

and never reversed these salutations. To Sir B. J. Sulivan's father,

he used to add to the " good morning" a short sentence, which was

never once repeated after his father's death. He scolded violently a

strange dog which came into the room through the open window; and he

scolded another parrot (saying "you naughty polly") which had got

out of its cage, and was eating apples on the kitchen table. See also,

to the same effect, Houzeau on parrots, Facultes Mentales, tom. ii.,

p. 309. Dr. A. Moschkau informs me that he knew a starling which never

made a mistake in saying in German " good morning" to persons

arriving, and "good bye, old fellow," to those departing. I could

add several other such cases.



  As Horne Tooke, one of the founders of the noble science of

philology, observes, language is an art, like brewing or baking; but

writing would have been a better simile. It certainly is not a true

instinct, for every language has to be learnt. It differs, however,

widely from all ordinary arts, for man has an instinctive tendency

to speak, as we see in the babble of our young children; whilst no

child has an instinctive tendency to brew, bake, or write. Moreover,

no philologist now supposes that any language has been deliberately

invented; it has been slowly and unconsciously developed by many

steps.* The sounds uttered by birds offer in several respects the

nearest analogy to language, for all the members of the same species

utter the same instinctive cries expressive of their emotions; and all

the kinds which sing, exert their power instinctively; but the

actual song, and even the call-notes, are learnt from their parents or

foster-parents. These sounds, as Daines Barrington*(2) has proved,

"are no more innate than language is in man." The first attempts to

sing "may be compared to the imperfect endeavour in a child to

babble." The young males continue practising, or as the

bird-catchers say, "recording," for ten or eleven months. Their

first essays show hardly a rudiment of the future song; but as they

grow older we can perceive what they are aiming at; and at last they

are said "to sing their song round." Nestlings which have learnt the

song of a distinct species, as with the canary-birds educated in the

Tyrol, teach and transmit their new song to their offspring. The

slight natural differences of song in the same species inhabiting

different districts may be appositely compared, as Barrington remarks,

"to provincial dialects"; and the songs of allied, though distinct

species may be compared with the languages of distinct races of man. I

have given the foregoing details to shew that an instinctive

tendency to acquire an art is not peculiar to man.



  * See some good remarks on this head by Prof. Whitney, in his

Oriental and Linguistic Studies, 1873, p. 354. He observes that the

desire of communication between man is the living force, which, in the

development of language, "works both consciously and unconsciously;

consciously as regards the immediate end to be attained; unconsciously

as regards the further consequences of the act."

  *(2) Hon. Daines Barrington in Philosoph. Transactions, 1773, p.

262. See also Dureau de la Malle, in Ann. des. Sc. Nat., 3rd series,

Zoolog., tom. x., p. 119.



  With respect to the origin of articulate language, after having read

on the one side the highly interesting works of Mr. Hensleigh

Wedgwood, the Rev. F. Farrar, and Prof. Schleicher,* and the

celebrated lectures of Prof. Max Muller on the other side, I cannot

doubt that language owes its origin to the imitation and

modification of various natural sounds, the voices of other animals,

and man's own instinctive cries, aided by signs and gestures. When

we treat of sexual selection we shall see that primeval man, or rather

some early progenitor of man, probably first used his voice in

producing true musical cadences, that is in singing, as do some of the

gibbon-apes at the present day; and we may conclude from a

widely-spread analogy, that this power would have been especially

exerted during the courtship of the sexes,- would have expressed

various emotions, such as love, jealousy, triumph,- and would have

served as a challenge to rivals. It is, therefore, probable that the

imitation of musical cries by articulate sounds may have given rise to

words expressive of various complex emotions. The strong tendency in

our nearest allies, the monkeys, in microcephalous idiots,*(2) and

in the barbarous races of mankind, to imitate whatever they hear

deserves notice, as bearing on the subject of imitation. Since monkeys

certainly understand much that is said to them by man, and when

wild, utter signal-cries of danger to their fellows;*(3) and since

fowls give distinct warnings for danger on the ground, or in the sky

from hawks (both, as well as a third cry, intelligible to dogs),*(4)

may not some unusually wise apelike animal have imitated the growl

of a beast of prey, and thus told his fellow-monkeys the nature of the

expected danger? This would have been a first step in the formation of

a language.



  * On the Origin of Language, by H. Wedgwood, 1866. Chapters on

Language, by the Rev. F. W. Farrar, 1865. These works are most

interesting. See also De la Phys. et de Parole, par Albert Lemoine,

1865, p. 190. The work on this subject, by the late Prof. Aug.

Schleicher, has been translated by Dr. Bikkers into English, under the

title of Darwinism tested by the Science of Language, 1869.

  *(2) Vogt, Memoire sur les Microcephales, 1867, p. 169. With respect

to savages, I have given some facts in my Journal of Researches,

&c., 1845, p. 206.

  *(3) See clear evidence on this head in the two works so often

quoted, by Brehm and Rengger.

  *(4) Houzeau gives a very curious account of his observations on

this subject in his Facultes Mentales des Animaux, tom. ii., p. 348.



  As the voice was used more and more, the vocal organs would have

been strengthened and perfected through the principle of the inherited

effects of use; and this would have reacted on the power of speech.

But the relation between the continued use of language and the

development of the brain, has no doubt been far more important. The

mental powers in some early progenitor of man must have been more

highly developed than in any existing ape, before even the most

imperfect form of speech could have come into use; but we may

confidently believe that the continued use and advancement of this

power would have reacted on the mind itself, by enabling and

encouraging it to carry on long trains of thought. A complex train

of thought can no more be carried on without the aid of words, whether

spoken or silent, than a long calculation without the use of figures

or algebra. It appears, also, that even an ordinary train of thought

almost requires, or is greatly facilitated by some form of language,

for the dumb, deaf, and blind girl, Laura Bridgman, was observed to

use her fingers whilst dreaming.* Nevertheless, a long succession of

vivid and connected ideas may pass through the mind without the aid of

any form of language, as we may infer from the movements of dogs

during their dreams. We have, also, seen that animals are able to

reason to a certain extent, manifestly without the aid of language.

The intimate connection between the brain, as it is now developed in

us, and the faculty of speech, is well shewn by those curious cases of

brain-disease in which speech is specially affected, as when the power

to remember substantives is lost, whilst other words can be

correctly used, or where substantives of a certain class, or all

except the initial letters of substantives and proper names are

forgotten.*(2) There is no more improbability in the continued use

of the mental and vocal organs leading to inherited changes in their

structure and functions, than in the case of hand-writing, which

depends partly on the form of the hand and partly on the disposition

of the mind; and handwriting is certainly inherited.*(3)



  * See remarks on this head by Dr. Maudsley, The Physiology and

Pathology of Mind, 2nd ed., 1868, p. 199.

  *(2) Many curious cases have been recorded. See, for instance, Dr.

Bateman On Aphasia, 1870, pp. 27, 31, 53, 100, &c. Also, Inquiries

Concerning the Intellectual Powers, by Dr. Abercrombie, 1838, p. 150.

  *(3) The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol.

ii., p. 6.



  Several writers, more especially Prof. Max Muller,* have lately

insisted that the use of language implies the power of forming general

concepts; and that as no animals are supposed to possess this power,

an impassable barrier is formed between them and man.*(2) With respect

to animals, I have already endeavoured to shew that they have this

power, at least in a rude and incipient degree. As far as concerns

infants of from ten to eleven months old, and deaf-mutes, it seems

to me incredible, that they should be able to connect certain sounds

with certain general ideas as quickly as they do, unless such ideas

were already formed in their minds. The same remark may be extended to

the more intelligent animals; as Mr. Leslie Stephen observes,*(3) "A

dog frames a general concept of cats or sheep, and knows the

corresponding words as well as a philosopher. And the capacity to

understand is as good a proof of vocal intelligence, though in an

inferior degree, as the capacity to speak."



  * Lectures on Mr. Darwin's Philosophy of Language, 1873.

  *(2) The judgment of a distinguished philologist, such as Prof.

Whitney, will have far more weight on this point than anything that

I can say. He remarks (Oriental and Linguistic Studies, 1873, p. 297),

in speaking of Bleek's views: "Because on the grand scale language

is the necessary auxiliary of thought, indispensable to the

development of the power of thinking, to the distinctness and

variety and complexity of cognitions, to the full mastery of

consciousness; therefore he would fain make thought absolutely

impossible without speech, identifying the faculty with its

instrument. He might just as reasonably assert that the human hand

cannot act without a tool. With such a doctrine to start from, he

cannot stop short of Max Muller's worst paradoxes, that an infant

(in fans, not speaking) is not a human being, and that deaf-mutes do

not become possessed of reason until they learn to twist their fingers

into imitation of spoken words." Max Muller gives in italics (Lectures

on Mr. Darwin's Philosophy of Language, 1873, third lecture) this

aphorism: "There is no thought without words, as little as there are

words without thought." What a strange definition must here be given

to the word thought!

  *(3) Essays on Free Thinking, &c., 1873, p. 82.



  Why the organs now used for speech should have been originally

perfected for this purpose, rather than any other organs, it is not

difficult to see. Ants have considerable powers of inter-communication

by means of their antennae, as shewn by Huber, who devotes a whole

chapter to their language. We might have used our fingers as efficient

instruments, for a person with practice can report to a deaf man every

word of a speech rapidly delivered at a public meeting; but the loss

of our hands, whilst thus employed, would have been a serious

inconvenience. As all the higher mammals possess vocal organs,

constructed on the same general plan as ours, and used as a means of

communication, it was obviously probable that these same organs

would be still further developed if the power of communication had

to be improved; and this has been effected by the aid of adjoining and

well adapted parts, namely the tongue and lips.* The fact of the

higher apes not using their vocal organs for speech, no doubt

depends on their intelligence not having been sufficiently advanced.

The possession by them of organs, which with long-continued practice

might have been used for speech, although not thus used, is paralleled

by the case of many birds which possess organs fitted for singing,

though they never sing. Thus, the nightingale and crow have vocal

organs similarly constructed, these being used by the former for

diversified song, and by the latter only for croaking.*(2) If it be

asked why apes have not had their intellects developed to the same

degree as that of man, general causes only can be assigned in

answer, and it is unreasonable to expect any thing more definite,

considering our ignorance with respect to the successive stages of

development through which each creature has passed.



  * See some good remarks to this effect by Dr. Maudsley, The

Physiology and Pathology of Mind, 1868, p. 199.

  *(2) Macgillivray, Hist. of British Birds, vol. ii., 1839, p. 29. An

excellent observer, Mr. Blackwall remarks that the magpie learns to

pronounce single words, and even short sentences, more readily than

almost any other British bird; yet, as he adds, after long and closely

investigating its habits, he has never known it, in a state of nature,

display any unusual capacity for imitation. Researches in Zoology,

1834, p. 158.



  The formation of different languages and of distinct species, and

the proofs that both have been developed through a gradual process,

are curiously parallel.* But we can trace the formation of many

words further back than that of species, for we can perceive how

they actually arose from the imitation of various sounds. We find in

distinct languages striking homologies due to community of descent,

and analogies due to a similar process of formation. The manner in

which certain letters or sounds change when others change is very like

correlated growth. We have in both cases the re-duplication of

parts, the effects of long-continued use, and so forth. The frequent

presence of rudiments, both in languages and in species, is still more

remarkable. The letter m in the word am, means I; so that in the

expression I am, a superfluous and useless rudiment has been retained.

In the spelling also of words, letters often remain as the rudiments

of ancient forms of pronunciation. Languages, like organic beings, can

be classed in groups under groups; and they can be classed either

naturally according to descent, or artificially by other characters.

Dominant languages and dialects spread widely, and lead to the gradual

extinction of other tongues. A language, like a species, when once

extinct, never, as Sir C. Lyell remarks, reappears. The same

language never has two birth-places. Distinct languages may be crossed

or blended together.*(2) We see variability in every tongue, and new

words are continually cropping up; but as there is a limit to the

powers of the memory, single words, like whole languages, gradually

become extinct. As Max Muller*(3) has well remarked:- "A struggle

for life is constantly going on amongst the words and grammatical

forms in each language. The better, the shorter, the easier forms

are constantly gaining the upper hand, and they owe their success to

their own inherent virtue." To these more important causes of the

survival of certain words, mere novelty and fashion may be added;

for there is in the mind of man a strong love for slight changes in

all things. The survival or preservation of certain favoured words

in the struggle for existence is natural selection.



  * See the very interesting parallelism between the development of

species and languages, given by Sir C. Lyell in The Geological

Evidences of the Antiquity of Man, 1863, chap. xxiii.

  *(2) See remarks to this effect by the Rev. F. W. Farrar, in an

interesting article, entitled Philology and Darwinism," in Nature,

March 24, 1870, p. 528.

  *(3) Nature, January 6, 1870, p. 257.



  The perfectly regular and wonderfully complex construction of the

languages of many barbarous nations has often been advanced as a

proof, either of the divine origin of these languages, or of the

high art and former civilisation of their founders. Thus F. von

Schlegel writes: "In those languages which appear to be at the

lowest grade of intellectual culture, we frequently observe a very

high and elaborate degree of art in their grammatical structure.

This is especially the case with the Basque and the Lapponian, and

many of the American languages."* But it is assuredly an error to

speak of any language as an art, in the sense of its having been

elaborately and methodically formed. Philologists now admit that

conjugations, declensions, &c., originally existed as distinct

words, since joined together; and as such words express the most

obvious relations between objects and persons, it is not surprising

that they should have been used by the men of most races during the

earliest ages. With respect to perfection, the following

illustration will best shew how easily we may err: a crinoid sometimes

consists of no less than 150,000 pieces of shell,*(2) all arranged

with perfect symmetry in radiating lines; but a naturalist does not

consider an animal of this kind as more perfect than a bilateral one

with comparatively few parts, and with none of these parts alike,

excepting on the opposite sides of the body. He justly considers the

differentiation and specialisation of organs as the test of

perfection. So with languages: the most symmetrical and complex

ought not to be ranked above irregular, abbreviated, and bastardised

languages, which have borrowed expressive words and useful forms of

construction from various conquering, conquered, or immigrant races.



  * Quoted by C. S. Wake, Chapters on Man, 1868, p. 101.

  *(2) Buckland, Bridgewater Treatise, p. 411.



  From these few and imperfect remarks I conclude that the extremely

complex and regular construction of many barbarous languages, is no

proof that they owe their origin to a special act of creation.* Nor,

as we have seen, does the faculty of articulate speech in itself offer

any insuperable objection to the belief that man has been developed

from some lower form.



  * See some good remarks on the simplification of languages, by Sir

J. Lubbock, Origin of Civilisation, 1870, p. 278.



  Sense of Beauty.- This sense has been declared to be peculiar to

man. I refer here only to the pleasure given by certain colours,

forms, and sounds, and which may fairly be called a sense of the

beautiful; with cultivated men such sensations are, however,

intimately associated with complex ideas and trains of thought. When

we behold a male bird elaborately displaying his graceful plumes or

splendid colours before the female, whilst other birds, not thus

decorated, make no such display, it is impossible to doubt that she

admires the beauty of her male partner. As women everywhere deck

themselves with these plumes, the beauty of such ornaments cannot be

disputed. As we shall see later, the nests of humming-birds, and the

playing passages of bower-birds are tastefully ornamented with

gaily-coloured objects; and this shews that they must receive some

kind of pleasure from the sight of such things. With the great

majority of animals, however, the taste for the beautiful is confined,

as far as we can judge, to the attractions of the opposite sex. The

sweet strains poured forth by many male birds during the season of

love, are certainly admired by the females, of which fact evidence

will hereafter be given. If female birds had been incapable of

appreciating the beautiful colours, the ornaments, and voices of their

male partners, all the labour and anxiety exhibited by the latter in

displaying their charms before the females would have been thrown

away; and this it is impossible to admit. Why certain bright colours

should excite pleasure cannot, I presume, be explained, any more

than why certain flavours and scents are agreeable; but habit has

something to do with the result, for that which is at first unpleasant

to our senses, ultimately becomes pleasant, and habits are

inherited. With respect to sounds, Helmholtz has explained to a

certain extent on physiological principles, why harmonies and

certain cadences are agreeable. But besides this, sounds frequently

recurring at irregular intervals are highly disagreeable, as every one

will admit who has listened at night to the irregular flapping of a

rope on board ship. The same principle seems to come into play with

vision, as the eye prefers symmetry or figures with some regular

recurrence. Patterns of this kind are employed by even the lowest

savages as ornaments; and they have been developed through sexual

selection for the adornment of some male animals. Whether we can or

not give any reason for the pleasure thus derived from vision and

hearing, yet man and many of the lower animals are alike pleased by

the same colours, graceful shading and forms, and the same sounds.

  The taste for the beautiful, at least as far as female beauty is

concerned, is not of a special nature in the human mind; for it

differs widely in the different races of man, and is not quite the

same even in the different nations of the same race. Judging from

the hideous ornaments, and the equally hideous music admired by most

savages, it might be urged that their Aesthetic faculty was not so

highly developed as in certain animals, for instance, as in birds.

Obviously no animal would be capable of admiring such scenes as the

heavens at night, a beautiful landscape, or refined music; but such

high tastes are acquired through culture, and depend on complex

associations; they are not enjoyed by barbarians or by uneducated

persons.

  Many of the faculties, which have been of inestimable service to man

for his progressive advancement, such as the powers of the

imagination, wonder, curiosity, an undefined sense of beauty, a

tendency to imitation, and the love of excitement or novelty, could

hardly fail to lead to capricious changes of customs and fashions. I

have alluded to this point, because a recent writer* has oddly fixed

on Caprice "as one of the most remarkable and typical differences

between savages and brutes." But not only can we partially

understand how it is that man is from various conflicting influences

rendered capricious, but that the lower animals are, as we shall

hereafter see, likewise capricious in their affections, aversions, and

sense of beauty. There is also reason to suspect that they love

novelty, for its own sake.



  * The Spectator, Dec. 4. 1869, p. 1430.



  Belief in God- Religion.- There is no evidence that man was

aboriginally endowed with the ennobling belief in the existence of

an Omnipotent God. On the contrary there is ample evidence, derived

not from hasty travellers, but from men who have long resided with

savages, that numerous races have existed, and still exist, who have

no idea of one or more gods, and who have no words in their

languages to express such an idea.* The question is of course wholly

distinct from that higher one, whether there exists a Creator and

Ruler of the universe; and this has been answered in the affirmative

by some of the highest intellects that have ever existed.



  * See an excellent article on this subject by the Rev. F. W. Farrar,

in the Anthropological Review, Aug., 1864, p. ccxvii. For further

facts see Sir J. Lubbock, Prehistoric Times, 2nd ed., 1869, p. 564;

and especially the chapters on Religion in his Origin of Civilisation,

1870.



  If, however, we include under the term "religion" the belief in

unseen or spiritual agencies the case is wholly different; for this

belief seems to be universal with the less civilised races. Nor is

it difficult to comprehend how it arose. As soon as the important

faculties of the imagination, wonder, and curiosity, together with

some power of reasoning, had become partially developed, man would

naturally crave to understand what was passing around him, and would

have vaguely speculated on his own existence. As Mr. M'Lennan* has

remarked, "Some explanation of the phenomena of life, a man must feign

for himself, and to judge from the universality of it, the simplest

hypothesis, and the first to occur to men, seems to have been that

natural phenomena are ascribable to the presence in animals, plants,

and things, and in the forces of nature, of such spirits prompting

to action as men are conscious they themselves possess." It is also

probable, as Mr. Tylor has shewn, that dreams may have first given

rise to the notion of spirits; for savages do not readily

distinguish between subjective and objective impressions. When a

savage dreams, the figures which appear before him are believed to

have come from a distance, and to stand over him; or "the soul of

the dreamer goes out on its travels, and comes home with a remembrance

of what it has seen."*(2) But until the faculties of imagination,

curiosity, reason, &c., had been fairly well developed in the mind

of man, his dreams would not have led him to believe in spirits, any

more than in the case of a dog.



  * "The Worship of Animals and Plants," in the Fortnightly Review,

Oct. 1, 1869, p. 422.

  *(2) Tylor, Early History of Mankind, 1865, p. 6. See also the three

striking chapters on the "Development of Religion," in Lubbock's

Origin of Civilisation, 1870. In a like manner Mr. Herbert Spencer, in

his ingenious essay in the Fortnightly Review (May 1, 1870, p. 535),

accounts for the earliest forms of religious belief throughout the

world, by man being led through dreams, shadows, and other causes,

to look at himself as a double essence, corporeal and spiritual. As

the spiritual being is supposed to exist after death and to be

powerful, it is propitiated by various gifts and ceremonies, and its

aid invoked. He then further shews that names or nicknames given

from some animal or other object, to the early progenitors or founders

of a tribe, are supposed after a long interval to represent the real

progenitor of the tribe; and such animal or object is then naturally

believed still to exist as a spirit, is held sacred, and worshipped as

a god. Nevertheless I cannot but suspect that there is a still earlier

and ruder stage, when anything which manifests power or movement is

thought to be endowed with some form of life, and with mental

faculties analogous to our own.



  The tendency in savages to imagine that natural objects and agencies

are animated by spiritual or living essences, is perhaps illustrated

by a little fact which I once noticed: my dog, a full-grown and very

sensible animal, was lying on the lawn during a hot and still day; but

at a little distance a slight breeze occasionally moved an open

parasol, which would have been wholly disregarded by the dog, had

any one stood near it. As it was, every time that the parasol slightly

moved, the dog growled fiercely and barked. He must, I think, have

reasoned to himself in a rapid and unconscious manner, that movement

without any apparent cause indicated the presence of some strange

living agent, and that no stranger had a right to be on his territory.

  The belief in spiritual agencies would easily pass into the belief

in the existence of one or more gods. For savages would naturally

attribute to spirits the same passions, the same love of vengeance

or simplest form of justice, and the same affections which they

themselves feel. The Fuegians appear to be in this respect in an

intermediate condition, for when the surgeon on board the Beagle

shot some young ducklings as specimens, York Minster declared in the

most solemn manner, "Oh, Mr. Bynoe, much rain, much snow, blow

much"; and this was evidently a retributive punishment for wasting

human food. So again he related how, when his brother killed a "wild

man," storms long raged, much rain and snow fell. Yet we could never

discover that the Fuegians believed in what we should call a God, or

practised any religious rites; and Jemmy Button, with justifiable

pride, stoutly maintained that there was no devil in his land. This

latter assertion is the more remarkable, as with savages the belief in

bad spirits is far more common than that in good ones.

  The feeling of religious devotion is a highly complex one,

consisting of love, complete submission to an exalted and mysterious

superior, a strong sense of dependence,* fear, reverence, gratitude,

hope for the future, and perhaps other elements. No being could

experience so complex an emotion until advanced in his intellectual

and moral faculties to at least a moderately high level. Nevertheless,

we see some distant approach to this state of mind in the deep love of

a dog for his master, associated with complete submission, some

fear, and perhaps other feelings. The behaviour of a dog when

returning to his master after an absence, and, as I may add, of a

monkey to his beloved keeper, is widely different from that towards

their fellows. In the latter case the transports of joy appear to be

somewhat less, and the sense of equality is shewn in every action.

Professor Braubach goes so far as to maintain that a dog looks on

his master as on a god.*(2)



  * See an able article on the "Physical Elements of Religion," by Mr.

L. Owen Pike, in Anthropological Review, April, 1870, p. lxiii.

  *(2) Religion, Moral, &c., der Darwin'schen Art-Lehre, 1869, s.

53. It is said (Dr. W. Lauder Lindsay, Journal of Mental Science,

1871, p. 43), that Bacon long ago, and the poet Burns, held the same

notion.



  The same high mental faculties which first led man to believe in

unseen spiritual agencies, then in fetishism, polytheism, and

ultimately in monotheism, would infallibly lead him, as long as his

reasoning powers remained poorly developed, to various strange

superstitions and customs. Many of these are terrible to think of-

such as the sacrifice of human beings to a blood-loving god; the trial

of innocent persons by the ordeal of poison or fire; witchcraft,

&c.- yet it is well occasionally to reflect on these superstitions,

for they shew us what an infinite debt of gratitude we owe to the

improvement of our reason, to science, and to our accumulated

knowledge. As Sir J. Lubbock* has well observed, "it is not too much

to say that the horrible dread of unknown evil hangs like a thick

cloud over savage life, and embitters every pleasure." These miserable

and indirect consequences of our highest faculties may be compared

with the incidental and occasional mistakes of the instincts of the

lower animals.



  * Prehistoric Times, 2nd ed., p. 571. In this work (p. 571) there

will be found an excellent account of the many strange and

capricious customs of savages.




Search the Secular Web | Bookstore | What's new? | Add an URL | Send Feedback | Disclaimer

[Internet Infidels] [Freethought Ring] [Church-State Ring]  [Free Speech Online]

Copyright © Internet Infidels 1995-1997. All rights reserved.

Last updated: Friday, 30-Jan-98 10:45:15 MST