Permanent
E-mail Addresses Freethought Audio
Tapes Products for Freethinkers

Library: Historical Documents: Charles Darwin: Descent of Man: Chapter 7


This file has been made available by the Bank Of Wisdom.

Descent of Man [ 1871 ]

Charles Darwin [ 1809 - 1882 ]

 

Chapter VII - On the Races of Man




  IT is not my intention here to describe the several so-called

races of men; but I am about to enquire what is the value of the

differences between them under a classificatory point of view, and how

they have originated. In determining whether two or more allied

forms ought to be ranked as species or varieties, naturalists are

practically guided by the following considerations; namely, the amount

of difference between them, and whether such differences relate to few

or many points of structure, and whether they are of physiological

importance; but more especially whether they are constant. Constancy

of character is what is chiefly valued and sought for by

naturalists. Whenever it can be shewn, or rendered probable, that

the forms in question have remained distinct for a long period, this

becomes an argument of much weight in favour of treating them as

species. Even a slight degree of sterility between any two forms

when first crossed, or in their offspring, is generally considered

as a decisive test of their specific distinctness; and their continued

persistence without blending within the same area, is usually accepted

as sufficient evidence, either of some degree of mutual sterility,

or in the case of animals of some mutual repugnance to pairing.

  Independently of fusion from intercrossing, the complete absence, in

a well-investigated region, of varieties linking together any two

closely-allied forms, is probably the most important of all the

criterions of their specific distinctness; and this is a somewhat

different consideration from mere constancy of character, for two

forms may be highly variable and yet not yield intermediate varieties.

Geographical distribution is often brought into play unconsciously and

sometimes consciously; so that forms living in two widely separated

areas, in which most of the other inhabitants are specifically

distinct, are themselves usually looked at as distinct; but in truth

this affords no aid in distinguishing geographical races from

so-called good or true species.

    Now let us apply these generally-admitted principles to the

races of man, viewing him in the same spirit as a naturalist would any

other animal. In regard to the amount of difference between the races,

we must make some allowance for our nice powers of discrimination

gained by the long habit of observing ourselves. In India, as

Elphinstone remarks, although a newly-arrived European cannot at first

distinguish the various native races, yet they soon appear to him

extremely dissimilar;* and the Hindoo cannot at first perceive any

difference between the several European nations. Even the most

distinct races of man are much more like each other in form than would

at first be supposed; certain negro tribes must be excepted, whilst

others, as Dr. Rohlfs writes to me, and as I have myself seen, have

Caucasian features. This general similarity is well shewn by the

French photographs in the Collection Anthropologique du Museum de

Paris of the men belonging to various races, the greater number of

which might pass for Europeans, as many persons to whom I have shewn

them have remarked. Nevertheless, these men, if seen alive, would

undoubtedly appear very distinct, so that we are clearly much

influenced in our judgment by the mere colour of the skin and hair, by

slight differences in the features, and by expression.



  * History of India, 1841, vol. i., p. 323. Father Ripa makes exactly

the same remark with respect to the Chinese.



   There is, however, no doubt that the various races, when

carefully compared and measured, differ much from each other,- as in

the texture of the hair, the relative proportions of all parts of

the body,* the capacity of the lungs, the form and capacity of the

skull, and even in the convolutions of the brain.*(2) But it would

be an endless task to specify the numerous points of difference. The

races differ also in constitution, in acclimatisation and in liability

to certain diseases. Their mental characteristies are likewise very

distinct; chiefly as it would appear in their emotional, but partly in

their intellectual faculties. Every one who has had the opportunity of

comparison, must have been struck with the contrast between the

taciturn, even morose, aborigines of S. America and the

lighthearted, talkative negroes. There is a nearly similar contrast

between the Malays and the Papuans,*(3) who live under the same

physical conditions, and are separated from each other only by a

narrow space of sea.



  * A vast number of measurements of whites, blacks, and Indians,

are given in the Investigations in the Military and Anthropolog.

Statistics of American Soldiers by B. A. Gould, 1869, pp. 298-358; "On

the capacity of the lungs," p. 471. See also the numerous and valuable

tables, by Dr. Weisbach, from the observations of Dr. Scherzer and Dr.

Schwarz, in the Reise der Novara: Anthropolog. Theil, 1867.

  *(2) See, for instance, Mr. Marshall's account of the brain of a

bushwoman, in Philosophical Transactions, 1864, p. 519.

  *(3) Wallace The Malay Archipelago, vol. ii., 1869, p. 178.



  We will first consider the arguments which may be advanced in favour

of classing the races of man as distinct species, and then the

arguments on the other side. If a naturalist, who had never before

seen a Negro, Hottentot, Australian, or Mongolian, were to compare

them, he would at once perceive that they differed in a multitude of

characters, some of slight and some of considerable importance. On

enquiry he would find that they were adapted to live under widely

different climates, and that they differed somewhat in bodily

constitution and mental disposition. If he were then told that

hundreds of similar specimens could be brought from the same

countries, he would assuredly declare that they were as good species

as many to which he had been in the habit of affixing specific

names. This conclusion would be greatly strengthened as soon as he had

ascertained that these forms had all retained the same character for

many centuries; and that negroes, apparently identical with existing

negroes, had lived at least 4000 years ago.* He would also hear, on

the authority of an excellent observer, Dr. Lund,*(2) that the human

skulls found in the caves of Brazil entombed with many extinct

mammals, belonged to the same type as that now prevailing throughout

the American continent.



  * With respect to the figures in the famous Egyptian caves of

Abou-Simbel, M. Pouchet says (The Plurality of the Human Races, Eng.

translat., 1864, p. 50), that he was far from finding recognisable

representations of the dozen or more nations which some authors

believe that they can recognise. Even some of the most strongly-marked

races cannot be identified with that degree of unanimity which might

have been expected from what has been written on the subject. Thus

Messrs. Nott and Gliddon (Types of Mankind, p. 148), state that

Rameses II, or the Great, has features superbly European; whereas

Knox, another firm believer in the specific distinctness of the

races of man (Races of Man, 1850, p. 201), speaking of young Memnon

(the same as Rameses II, as I am informed by Mr. Birch), insists in

the strongest manner that he is identical in character with the Jews

of Antwerp. Again, when I looked at the statue of Amunoph III, I

agreed with two officers of the establishment, both competent

judges, that he had a strongly-marked negro type of features; but

Messrs. Nott and Gliddon (ibid., p. 146, fig. 53), describe him as a

hybrid, but not of "negro intermixture."

  *(2) As quoted by Nott and Gliddon, Types of Mankind, 1854, p.

439. They give also corroborative evidence; but C. Vogt thinks that

the subject requires further investigation.



  Our naturalist would then perhaps turn to geographical distribution,

and he would probably declare that those forms must be distinct

species, which differ not only in appearance, but are fitted for

hot, as well as damp or dry countries, and for the arctic regions.

He might appeal to the fact that no species in the group next to

man- namely, the Quadrumana, can resist a low temperature, or any

considerable change of climate; and that the species which come

nearest to man have never been reared to maturity, even under the

temperate climate of Europe. He would be deeply impressed with the

fact, first noticed by Agassiz,* that the different races of man are

distributed over the world in the same zoological provinces, as

those inhabited by undoubtedly distinct species and genera of mammals.

This is manifestly the case with the Australian, Mongolian, and

Negro races of man; in a less well-marked manner with the

Hottentots; but plainly with the Papuans and Malays, who are

separated, as Mr. Wallace has shewn, by nearly the same line which

divides the great Malayan and Australian zoological provinces. The

aborigines of America range throughout the continent; and this at

first appears opposed to the above rule, for most of the productions

of the Southern and Northern halves differ widely: yet some few living

forms, as the opossum, range from the one into the other, as did

formerly some of the gigantic Edentata. The Esquimaux, like other

arctic animals, extend round the whole polar regions. It should be

observed that the amount of difference between the mammals of the

several zoological provinces does not correspond with the degree of

separation between the latter; so that it can hardly be considered

as an anomaly that the Negro differs more, and the American much

less from the other races of man, than do the mammals of the African

and American continents from the mammals of the other provinces.

Man, it may be added, does not appear to have aboriginally inhabited

any oceanic island; and in this respect, he resembles the other

members of his class.



  * "Diversity of Origin of the Human Races," in the Christian

Examiner, July, 1850.



  In determining whether the supposed varieties of the same kind of

domestic animal should be ranked as such, or as specifically distinct,

that is, whether any of them are descended from distinct wild species,

every naturalist would lay much stress on the fact of their external

parasites being specifically distinct. All the more stress would be

laid on this fact, as it would be an exceptional one; for I am

informed by Mr. Denny that the most different kinds of dogs, fowls,

and pigeons, in England, are infested by the same species of

Pediculi or lice. Now Mr. A. Murray has carefully examined the

Pediculi. collected in different countries from the different races of

man;* and he finds that they differ, not only in colour, but in the

structure of their claws and limbs. In every case in which many

specimens were obtained the differences were constant. The surgeon

of a whaling ship in the Pacific assured me that when the Pediculi,

with which some Sandwich Islanders on board swarmed, strayed on to the

bodies of the English sailors, they died in the course of three or

four days. These Pediculi were darker coloured, and appeared different

from those proper to the natives of Chiloe in South America, of

which he gave me specimens. These, again, appeared larger and much

softer than European lice. Mr. Murray procured four kinds from Africa,

namely, from the Negroes of the Eastern and Western coasts, from the

Hottentots and Kaffirs; two kinds from the natives of Australia; two

from North and two from South America. In these latter cases it may be

presumed that the Pediculi came from natives inhabiting different

districts. With insects slight structural differences, if constant,

are generally esteemed of specific value: and the fact of the races of

man being infested by parasites, which appear to be specifically

distinct, might fairly be urged as an argument that the races

themselves ought to be classed as distinct species.



  * Transactions of the Royal Society of Edinburgh, vol. xxii, 1861,

p. 567.



  Our supposed naturalist having proceeded thus far in his

investigation, would next enquire whether the races of men, when

crossed, were in any degree sterile. He might consult the work* of

Professor Broca, a cautious and philosophical observer, and in this he

would find good evidence that some races were quite fertile

together, but evidence of an opposite nature in regard to other races.

Thus it has been asserted that the native women of Australia and

Tasmania rarely produce children to European men; the evidence,

however, on this head has now been shewn to be almost valueless. The

half-castes are killed by the pure blacks: and an account has lately

been published of eleven half-caste youths murdered and burnt at the

same time, whose remains were found by the police.*(2) Again, it has

often been said that when mulattoes intermarry, they produce few

children; on the other hand, Dr. Bachman, of Charleston,*(3)

positively asserts that he has known mulatto families which have

intermarried for several generations, and have continued on an average

as fertile as either pure whites or pure blacks. Enquiries formerly

made by Sir C. Lyell on this subject led him, as he informs me, to the

same conclusion.*(4) In the United States the census for the year 1854

included, according to Dr. Bachman, 405,751 mulattoes; and this

number, considering all the circumstances of the case, seems small;

but it may partly be accounted for by the degraded and anomalous

position of the class, and by the profligacy of the women. A certain

amount of absorption of mulattoes into negroes must always be in

progress; and this would lead to an apparent diminution of the former.

The inferior vitality of mulattoes is spoken of in a trustworthy

work*(5) as a well-known phenomenon; and this, although a different

consideration from their lessened fertility, may perhaps be advanced

as a proof of the specific distinctness of the parent races. No

doubt both animal and vegetable hybrids, when produced from

extremely distinct species, are liable to premature death; but the

parents of mulattoes cannot be put under the category of extremely

distinct species. The common mule, so notorious for long life and

vigour, and yet so sterile, shews how little necessary connection

there is in hybrids between lessened fertility and vitality; other

analogous cases could be cited.



  * On the Phenomena of Hybridity in the Genus Homo, Eng. translat.,

1864.

  *(2) See the interesting letter by Mr. T. A. Murray, in the

Anthropological Review, April, 1868, p. liii. In this letter Count

Strzelecki's statement that Australian women who have borne children

to a white man, are afterwards sterile with their own race, is

disproved. M. A. de Quatrefages has also collected (Revue des Cours

Scientifiques, March, 1869, p. 239), much evidence that Australians

and Europeans are not sterile when crossed.

  *(3) An Examination of Prof. Agassiz's Sketch of the Nat.

Provinces of the Animal World, Charleston, 1855, p. 44.

  *(4) Dr. Rohlfs writes to me that he found the mixed races in the

Great Sahara, derived from Arabs, Berbers, and Negroes of three

tribes, extraordinarily fertile. On the other hand, Mr. Winwood

Reade informs me that the Negroes on the Gold Coast, though admiring

white men and mulattoes, have a maxim that mulattoes should not

intermarry, as the children are few and sickly. This belief, as Mr.

Reade remarks, deserves attention, as white men have visited and

resided on the Gold Coast for four hundred years, so that the

natives have had ample time to gain knowledge through experience.

  *(5) Military and Anthropological Statistics of American Soldiers,

by B. A. Gould, 1869, p. 319.



  Even if it should hereafter be proved that all the races of men were

perfectly fertile together, he who was inclined from other reasons

to rank them as distinct species, might with justice argue that

fertility and sterility are not safe criterions of specific

distinctness. We know that these qualities are easily affected by

changed conditions of life, or by close interbreeding, and that they

are governed by highly complex laws, for instance, that of the unequal

fertility of converse crosses between the same two species. With forms

which must be ranked as undoubted species, a perfect series exists

from those which are absolutely sterile when crossed, to those which

are almost or completely fertile. The degrees of sterility do not

coincide strictly with the degrees of difference between the parents

in external structures or habits of life. Man in many respects may

be compared with those animals which have long been domesticated,

and a large body of evidence can be advanced in favour of the

Pallasian doctrine,* that domestication tends to eliminate the

sterility which is so general a result of the crossing of species in a

state of nature. From these several considerations, it may be justly

urged that the perfect fertility of the intercrossed races of man,

if established, would not absolutely preclude us from ranking them

as distinct species.



  * The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol. ii

p. 109. I may here remind the reader that the sterility of species

when crossed is not a specially acquired quality, but, like the

incapacity of certain trees to be grafted together, is incidental on

other acquired differences. The nature these differences is unknown,

but they relate more especially to the reproductive system, and much

less so to external structure or to ordinary differences in

constitution. One important element in the sterility of crossed

species apparently lies in one or both having been long habituated

to fixed conditions; for we know that changed conditions have a

special influence on the reproductive system, and we have good

reason to believe (as before remarked) that the fluctuating conditions

of domestication tend to eliminate that sterility which is so

general with species, in a natural state, when crossed. It has

elsewhere been shewn by me (ibid., vol. ii., p. 185, and Origin of

Species, (OOS), that the sterility of crossed species has not been

acquired through natural selection: we can see that when two forms

have already been rendered very sterile, it is scarcely possible

that their sterility should be augmented by the preservation or

survival of the more and more sterile individuals; for, as the

sterility increases. fewer and fewer offspring will be produced from

which to breed, and at last only single individuals will be produced

at the rarest intervals. But there is even a higher grade of sterility

than this. Both Gartner and Kolreuter have proved that in genera of

plants, including many species, a series can be formed from species

which, when crossed, yield fewer and fewer seeds, to species which

never produce a single seed, but yet are affected by the pollen of the

other species, as shewn by the swelling of the germen. It is here

manifestly impossible to select the more sterile individuals, which

have already ceased to yield seeds; so that the acme of sterility,

when the germen alone is affected, cannot have been gained through

selection. This acme, and no doubt the other grades of sterility,

are the incidental results of certain unknown differences in the

constitution of the reproductive system of the species which are

crossed.



  Independently of fertility, the characters presented by the

offspring from a cross have been thought to indicate whether or not

the parent-forms ought to be ranked as species or varieties; but after

carefully studying the evidence, I have come to the conclusion that no

general rules of this kind can be trusted. The ordinary result of a

cross is the production of a blended or intermediate form; but in

certain cases some of the offspring take closely after one

parent-form, and some after the other. This is especially apt to occur

when the parents differ in characters which first appeared as sudden

variations or monstrosities.* I refer to this point, because Dr.

Rohlfs informs me that he has frequently seen in Africa the

offspring of negroes crossed with members of other races, either

completely black or completely white, or rarely piebald. On the

other hand, it is notorious that in America mulattoes commonly present

an intermediate appearance.



  * The Variation of Animals, &c., vol. ii., p. 92.



  We have now seen that a naturalist might feel himself fully

justified in ranking the races of man as distinct species; for he

has found that they are distinguished by many differences in structure

and constitution, some being of importance. These differences have,

also, remained nearly constant for very long periods of time. Our

naturalist will have been in some degree influenced by the enormous

range of man, which is a great anomaly in the class of mammals, if

mankind be viewed as a single species. He will have been struck with

the distribution of the several so-called races, which accords with

that of other undoubtedly distinct species of mammals. Finally, he

might urge that the mutual fertility of all the races has not as yet

been fully proved, and even if proved would not be an absolute proof

of their specific identity.

  On the other side of the question, if our supposed naturalist were

to enquire whether the forms of man keep distinct like ordinary

species, when mingled together in large numbers in the same country,

he would immediately discover that this was by no means the case. In

Brazil he would behold an immense mongrel population of Negroes and

Portuguese; in Chiloe, and other parts of South America, he would

behold the whole population consisting of Indians and Spaniards

blended in various degrees.* In many parts of the same continent he

would meet with the most complex crosses between Negroes, Indians, and

Europeans; and judging from the vegetable kingdom, such triple crosses

afford the severest test of the mutual fertility of the parent

forms. In one island of the Pacific he would find a small population

of mingled Polynesian and English blood; and in the Fiji Archipelago a

population of Polynesians and Negritos crossed in all degrees. Many

analogous cases could be added; for instance, in Africa. Hence the

races of man are not sufficiently distinct to inhabit the same country

without fusion; and the absence of fusion affords the usual and best

test of specific distinctness.



  * M. de Quatrefages has given (Anthropological Review, Jan., 1869,

p. 22), an interesting account of the success and energy of the

Paulistas in Brazil, who are a much crossed race of Portuguese and

Indians, with a mixture of the blood of other races.



  Our naturalist would likewise be much disturbed as soon as he

perceived that the distinctive characters of all the races were highly

variable. This fact strikes every one on first beholding the negro

slaves in Brazil, who have been imported from all parts of Africa. The

same remark holds good with the Polynesians, and with many other

races. It may be doubted whether any character can be named which is

distinctive of a race and is constant. Savages, even within the limits

of the same tribe, are not nearly so uniform in character, as has been

often asserted. Hottentot women offer certain peculiarities, more

strongly marked than those occurring in any other race, but these

are known not to be of constant occurrence. In the several American

tribes, colour and hairiness differ considerably; as does colour to

a certain degree, and the shape of the features greatly, in the

negroes of Africa. The shape of the skull varies much in some

races;* and so it is with every other character. Now all naturalists

have learnt by dearly bought experience, how rash it is to attempt

to define species by the aid of inconstant characters.



  * For instance, with the aborigines of America and Australia,

Prof. Huxley says (Transact. Internat. Congress of Prehist. Arch.,

1868, p. 105), that the skulls of many South Germans and Swiss are "as

short and as broad as those of the Tartars," &c.



  But the most weighty of all the arguments against treating the races

of man as distinct species, is that they graduate into each other,

independently in many cases, as far as we can judge, of their having

intercrossed. Man has been studied more carefully than any other

animal, and yet there is the greatest possible diversity amongst

capable judges whether he should be classed as a single species or

race, or as two (Virey), as three (Jacquinot), as four (Kant), five

(Blumenbach), six (Buffon), seven (Hunter), eight (Agassiz), eleven

(Pickering), fifteen (Bory de St-Vincent), sixteen (Desmoulins),

twenty-two (Morton), sixty (Crawfurd), or as sixty-three, according to

Burke.* This diversity of judgment does not prove that the races ought

not to be ranked as species, but it shews that they graduate into each

other, and that it is hardly possible to discover clear distinctive

characters between them.



  * See a good discussion on this subject in Waitz, Introduction to

Anthropology, Eng. translat., 1863, pp. 198-208, 227. I have taken

some of the above statements from H. Tuttle's Origin and Antiquity

of Physical Man, Boston, 1866, p. 35.



  Every naturalist who has had the misfortune to undertake the

description of a group of highly varying organisms, has encountered

cases (I speak after experience) precisely like that of man; and if of

a cautious disposition, he will end by uniting all the forms which

graduate into each other, under a single species; for he will say to

himself that he has no right to give names to objects which he

cannot define. Cases of this kind occur in the Order which include

man, namely in certain genera of monkeys; whilst in other genera, as

in Cercopithecus, most of the species can be determined with

certainty. In the American genus Cebus, the various forms are ranked

by some naturalists as species, by others as mere geographical

races. Now if numerous specimens of Cebus were collected from all

parts of South America, and those forms which at present appear to

be specifically distinct, were found to graduate into each other by

close steps, they would usually be ranked as mere varieties or

races; and this course has been followed by most naturalists with

respect to the races of man. Nevertheless, it must be confessed that

there are forms, at least in the vegetable kingdom,* which we cannot

avoid naming as species, but which are connected together by

numberless gradations, independently of intercrossing.



  * Prof. Nageli has carefully described several striking cases in his

Botanische Mittheilungen, B. ii., 1866, ss. 294-369. Prof. Asa Gray

has made analogous remarks on some intermediate forms in the

Compositae of N. America.



  Some naturalists have lately employed the term "sub-species" to

designate forms which possess many of the characteristics of true

species, but which hardly deserve so high a rank. Now if we reflect on

the weighty arguments above given, for raising the races of man to the

dignity of species, and the insuperable difficulties on the other side

in defining them, it seems that the term "sub-species" might here be

used with propriety. But from long habit the term "race" will

perhaps always be employed. The choice of terms is only so far

important in that it is desirable to use, as far as possible, the same

terms for the same degrees of difference. Unfortunately this can

rarely be done: for the larger genera generally include closely-allied

forms, which can be distinguished only with much difficulty, whilst

the smaller genera within the same family include forms that are

perfectly distinct; yet all must be ranked equally as species. So

again, species within the same large genus by no means resemble each

other to the same degree: on the contrary, some of them can

generally be arranged in little groups round other species, like

satellites round planets.*



  * Origin of Species. (OOS)



  The question whether mankind consists of one or several species

has of late years been much discussed by anthropologists, who are

divided into the two schools of monogenists and polygenists. Those who

do not admit the principle of evolution, must look at species as

separate creations, or in some manner as distinct entities; and they

must decide what forms of man they will consider as species by the

analogy of the method commonly pursued in ranking other organic beings

as species. But it is a hopeless endeavour to decide this point, until

some definition of the term "species" is generally accepted; and the

definition must not include an indeterminate element such as an act of

creation. We might as well attempt without any definition to decide

whether a certain number of houses should be called a village, town,

or city. We have a practical illustration of the difficulty in the

never-ending doubts whether many closely-allied mammals, birds,

insects, and plants, which represent each other respectively in

North America and Europe, should be ranked as species or

geographical races; and the like holds true of the productions of many

islands situated at some little distance from the nearest continent.

  Those naturalists, on the other hand, who admit the principle of

evolution, and this is now admitted by the majority of rising men,

will feel no doubt that all the races of man are descended from a

single primitive stock; whether or not they may think fit to designate

the races as distinct species, for the sake of expressing their amount

of difference.* With our domestic animals the question whether the

various races have arisen from one or more species is somewhat

different. Although it may be admitted that all the races, as well

as all the natural species within the same genus, have sprung from the

same primitive stock, yet it is a fit subject for discussion,

whether all the domestic races of the dog, for instance, have acquired

their present amount of difference since some one species was first

domesticated by man; or whether they owe some of their characters to

inheritance from distinct species, which had already been

differentiated in a state of nature. With man no such question can

arise, for he cannot be said to have been domesticated at any

particular period.



  * See Prof. Huxley to this effect in the Fortnightly Review, 1865,

p. 275.



  During an early stage in the divergence of the races of man from a

common stock, the differences between the races and their number

must have been small; consequently as far as their distinguishing

characters are concerned, they then had less claim to rank as distinct

species than the existing so-called races. Nevertheless, so

arbitrary is the term of species, that such early races would

perhaps have been ranked by some naturalists as distinct species, if

their differences, although extremely slight, had been more constant

than they are at present, and had not graduated into each other.

  It is however possible, though far from probable, that the early

progenitors of man might formerly have diverged much in character,

until they became more unlike each other than any now existing

races; but that subsequently, as suggested by Vogt,* they converged in

character. When man selects the offspring of two distinct species

for the same object, he sometimes induces a considerable amount of

convergence, as far as general appearance is concerned. This is the

case, as shown by von Nathusius,*(2) with the improved breeds of the

pig, which are descended from two distinct species; and in a less

marked manner with the improved breeds of cattle. A great anatomist,

Gratiolet, maintains that the anthropomorphous apes do not form a

natural sub-group; but that the orang is a highly developed gibbon

or Semnopithecus, the chimpanzee a highly developed Macacus, and the

gorilla a highly developed mandrill. If this conclusion, which rests

almost exclusively on brain-characters, be admitted, we should have

a case of convergence at least in external characters, for the

anthropomorphous apes are certainly more like each other in many

points, than they are to other apes. All analogical resemblances, as

of a whale to a fish, may indeed be said to be cases of convergence;

but this term has never been applied to superficial and adaptive

resemblances. It would, however be extremely rash to attribute to

convergence close similarity of character in many points of

structure amongst the modified descendants of widely distinct

beings. The form of a crystal is determined solely by the molecular

forces, and it is not surprising that dissimilar substances should

sometimes assume the same form; but with organic beings we should bear

in mind that the form of each depends on an infinity of complex

relations, namely on variations, due to causes far too intricate to be

followed,- on the nature of the variations preserved, these

depending on the physical conditions, and still more on the

surrounding organisms which compete with each,- and lastly, on

inheritance (in itself a fluctuating element) from innumerable

progenitors, all of which have had their forms determined through

equally complex relations. It appears incredible that the modified

descendants of two organisms, if these differed from each other in a

marked manner, should ever afterwards converge so closely as to lead

to a near approach to identity throughout their whole organisation. In

the case of the convergent races of pigs above referred to, evidence

of their descent from two primitive stock is, according to von

Nathusius, still plainly retained, in certain bones of their skulls.

If the races of man had descended, as is supposed by some naturalists,

from two or more species, which differed from each other as much, or

nearly as much, as does the orang from the gorilla, it can hardly be

doubted that marked differences in the structure of certain bones

would still be discoverable in man as he now exists.



  * Lectures on Man, Eng. translat., 1864, p. 468.

  *(2) Die Rassen des Schweines, 1860, s. 46. Vorstudien fur

Geschichte, &c., "Schweinesschadel," 1864, s. 104. With respect to

cattle, see M. de Quatrefages, Unite de l'Espece Humaine, 1861, p.

119.



  Although the existing races of man differ in many respects, as in

colour, hair, shape of skull, proportions of the body, &c., yet if

their whole structure be taken into consideration they are found to

resemble each other closely in a multitude of points. Many of these

are of so unimportant or of so singular a nature, that it is extremely

improbable that they should have been independently acquired by

aboriginally distinct species or races. The same remark holds good

with equal or greater force with respect to the numerous points of

mental similarity between the most distinct races of man. The American

aborigines, Negroes and Europeans are as different from each other

in mind as any three races that can be named; yet I was incessantly

struck, whilst living with the Feugians on board the Beagle, with

the many little traits of character, shewing how similar their minds

were to ours; and so it was with a full-blooded negro with whom I

happened once to be intimate.

  He who will read Mr. Tylor's and Sir J. Lubbock's interesting works*

can hardly fail to be deeply impressed with the close similarity

between the men of all races in tastes, dispositions and habits.

This is shown by the pleasure which they all take in dancing, rude

music, acting, painting, tattooing, and otherwise decorating

themselves; in their mutual comprehension of gesture-language, by

the same expression in their features, and by the same inarticulate

cries, when excited by the same emotions. This similarity, or rather

identity, is striking, when contrasted with the different

expressions and cries made by distinct species of monkeys. There is

good evidence that the art of shooting with bows and arrows has not

been handed down from any common progenitor of mankind, yet as

Westropp and Nilsson have remarked,*(2) the stone arrow-heads, brought

from the most distant parts of the world, and manufactured at the most

remote periods, are almost identical; and this fact can only be

accounted for by the various races having similar inventive or

mental powers. The same observation has been made by

archaeologists*(3) with respect to certain widely-prevalent ornaments,

such as zig-zags, &c.; and with respect to various simple beliefs

and customs, such as the burying of the dead under megalithic

structures. I remember observing in South America,*(4) that there,

as in so many other parts of the world, men have generally chosen

the summits of lofty hills, to throw up piles of stones, either as a

record of some remarkable event, or for burying their dead.



  * Tylor's Early History of Mankind, 1865: with respect to

gesture-language, see p. 54. Lubbock's Prehistoric Times, 2nd ed.,

1869.

  *(2) "On Analogous Forms of Implements," in Memoirs of

Anthropological Society by H. M. Westropp. The Primitive Inhabitants

of Scandinavia, Eng. translat., edited by Sir J. Lubbock, 1868, p.

104.

  *(3) Westropp "On Cromlechs," &c., Journal of Ethnological Soc.,

as given in Scientific Opinion, June 2, 1869, p. 3.

  *(4) Journal of Researches: Voyage of the Beagle, p. 46.



  Now when naturalists observe a close agreement in numerous small

details of habits, tastes, and dispositions between two or more

domestic races, or between nearly-allied natural forms, they use

this fact as an argument that they are descended from a common

progenitor who was thus endowed; and consequently that all should be

classed under the same species. The same argument may be applied

with much force to the races of man.

  As it is improbable that the numerous and unimportant points of

resemblance between the several races of man in bodily structure and

mental faculties (I do not here refer to similar customs) should all

have been independently acquired, they must have been inherited from

progenitors who had these same characters. We thus gain some insight

into the early state of man, before he had spread step by step over

the face of the earth. The spreading of man to regions widely

separated by the sea, no doubt, preceded any great amount of

divergence of character in the several races; for otherwise we

should sometimes meet with the same race in distinct continents; and

this is never the case. Sir J. Lubbock, after comparing the arts now

practised by savages in all parts of the world, specifies those

which man could not have known, when he first wandered from his

original birthplace; for if once learnt they would never have been

forgotten.* He thus shews that "the spear, which is but a

development of the knife-point, and the club, which is but a long

hammer, are the only things left." He admits, however, that the art of

making fire probably had been already discovered, for it is common

to all the races now existing, and was known to the ancient

cave-inhabitants of Europe. Perhaps the art of making rude canoes or

rafts was likewise known; but as man existed at a remote epoch, when

the land in many places stood at a very different level to what it

does now, he would have been able, without the aid of canoes, to

have spread widely. Sir J. Lubbock further remarks how improbable it

is that our earliest ancestors could have "counted as high as ten,

considering that so many races now in existence cannot get beyond

four." Nevertheless, at this early period, the intellectual and social

faculties of man could hardly have been inferior in any extreme degree

to those possessed at present by the lowest savages; otherwise

primeval man could not have been so eminently successful in the

struggle for life, as proved by his early and wide diffusion.



  * Prehistoric Times, 1869, p. 574.



  From the fundamental differences between certain languages, some

philologists have inferred that when man first became widely diffused,

he was not a speaking animal; but it may be suspected that

languages, far less perfect than any now spoken, aided by gestures,

might have been used, and yet have left no traces on subsequent and

more highly-developed tongues. Without the use of some language,

however imperfect, it appears doubtful whether man's intellect could

have risen to the standard implied by his dominant position at an

early period.

  Whether primeval man, when he possessed but few arts, and those of

the rudest kind, and when his power of language was extremely

imperfect, would have deserved to be called man, must depend on the

definition which we employ. In a series of forms graduating insensibly

from some ape-like creature to man as he now exists, it would be

impossible to fix on any definite point where the term "man" ought

to be used. But this is a matter of very little importance. So

again, it is almost a matter of indifference whether the so-called

races of man are thus designated, or are ranked as species or

sub-species; but the latter term appears the more appropriate.

Finally, we may conclude that when the principle of evolution is

generally accepted, as it surely will be before long, the dispute

between the monogenists and the polygenists will die a silent and

unobserved death.



  One other question ought not to be passed over without notice,

namely, whether, as is sometimes assumed, each sub-species or race

of man has sprung from a single pair of progenitors. With our domestic

animals a new race can readily be formed by carefully matching the

varying offspring from a single pair, or even from a single individual

possessing some new character; but most of our races have been formed,

not intentionally from a selected pair, but unconsciously by the

preservation of many individuals which have varied, however

slightly, in some useful or desired manner. If in one country stronger

and heavier horses, and in another country lighter and fleeter ones,

were habitually preferred, we may feel sure that two distinct

sub-breeds would be produced in the course of time, without any one

pair having been separated and bred from, in either country. Many

races have been thus formed, and their manner of formation is

closely analogous to that of natural species. We know, also, that

the horses taken to the Falkland Islands have, during successive

generations, become smaller and weaker, whilst those which have run

wild on the Pampas have acquired larger and coarser heads; and such

changes are manifestly due, not to any one pair, but to all the

individuals having been subjected to the same conditions, aided,

perhaps, by the principle of reversion. The new sub-breeds in such

cases are not descended from any single pair, but from many

individuals which have varied in different degrees, but in the same

general manner; and we may conclude that the races of man have been

similarly produced, the modifications being either the direct result

of exposure to different conditions, or the indirect result of some

form of selection. But to this latter subject we shall presently

return.

  On the Extinction of the Races of Man.- The partial or complete

extinction of many races and sub-races of man is historically known.

Humboldt saw in South America a parrot which was the sole living

creature that could speak a word of the language of a lost tribe.

Ancient monuments and stone implements found in all parts of the

world, about which no tradition has been preserved by the present

inhabitants, indicate much extinction. Some small and broken tribes,

remnants of former races, still survive in isolated and generally

mountainous districts. In Europe the ancient races were all, according

to Shaaffhausen,* "lower in the scale than the rudest living savages";

they must therefore have differed, to a certain extent, from any

existing race. The remains described by Professor Broca from Les

Eyzies, though they unfortunately appear to have belonged to a

single family, indicate a race with a most singular combination of low

or simious, and of high characteristics. This race is "entirely

different from any other, ancient or modern, that we have heard

of."*(2) It differed, therefore, from the quaternary race of the

caverns of Belgium.



  * Translation in Anthropological Review, Oct., 1868, p. 431.

  *(2) Transactions, International Congress of Prehistoric Archaeology

1868, pp. 172-175. See also Broca (tr.) in Anthropological Review,

Oct., 1868, p. 410.



  Man can long resist conditions which appear extremely unfavourable

for his existence.* He has long lived in the extreme regions of the

North, with no wood for his canoes or implements, and with only

blubber as fuel, and melted snow as drink. In the southern extremity

of America the Fuegians survive without the protection of clothes,

or of any building worthy to be called a hovel. In South Africa the

aborigines wander over arid plains, where dangerous beasts abound. Man

can withstand the deadly influence of the Terai at the foot of the

Himalaya, and the pestilential shores of tropical Africa.



  * Dr. Gerland, Uber das Aussterben der Naturvolker 1868, s. 82.



  Extinction follows chiefly from the competition of tribe with tribe,

and race with race. Various checks are always in action, serving to

keep down the numbers of each savage tribe,- such as periodical

famines, nomadic habits and the consequent deaths of infants,

prolonged suckling, wars, accidents, sickness, licentiousness, the

stealing of women, infanticide, and especially lessened fertility.

If any one of these checks increases in power, even slightly, the

tribe thus affected tends to decrease; and when of two adjoining

tribes one becomes less numerous and less powerful than the other, the

contest is soon settled by war, slaughter, cannibalism, slavery, and

absorption. Even when a weaker tribe is not thus abruptly swept

away, if it once begins to decrease, it generally goes on decreasing

until it becomes extinct.*



  * Gerland, ibid., s. 12, gives facts in support of this statement.



  When civilised nations come into contact with barbarians the

struggle is short, except where a deadly climate gives its aid to

the native race. Of the causes which lead to the victory of

civilised nations, some are plain and simple, others complex and

obscure. We can see that the cultivation of the land will be fatal

in many ways to savages, for they cannot, or will not, change their

habits. New diseases and vices have in some cases proved highly

destructive; and it appears that a new disease often causes much

death, until those who are most susceptible to its destructive

influence are gradually weeded out;* and so it may be with the evil

effects from spirituous liquors, as well as with the unconquerably

strong taste for them shewn by so many savages. It further appears,

mysterious as is the fact, that the first meeting of distinct and

separated people generates disease.*(2) Mr. Sproat, who in Vancouver

Island closely attended to the subject of extinction, believed that

changed habits of life, consequent on the advent of Europeans, induces

much ill health. He lays, also, great stress on the apparently

trifling cause that the natives become "bewildered and dull by the new

life around them; they lose the motives for exertion, and get no new

ones in their place."*(3)



  * See remarks to this effect in Sir H. Holland's Medical Notes and

Reflections, 1839, p. 390.

  *(2) I have collected (Journal of Researches: Voyage of the

Beagle, p. 435) a good many cases bearing on this subject; see also

Gerland, ibid., s. 8. Poeppig speaks of the "breath of civilisation as

poisonous to savages."

  *(3) Sproat, Scenes and Studies of Savage Life, 1868, p. 284.



  The grade of their civilisation seems to be a most important element

in the success of competing nations. A few centuries ago Europe feared

the inroads of Eastern barbarians; now any such fear would be

ridiculous. It is a more curious fact, as Mr. Bagehot has remarked,

that savages did not formerly waste away before the classical nations,

as they now do before modern civilised nations; had they done so,

the old moralists would have mused over the event; but there is no

lament in any writer of that period over the perishing barbarians.*

The most potent of all the causes of extinction, appears in many cases

to be lessened fertility and ill-health, especially amongst the

children, arising from changed conditions of life, notwithstanding

that the new conditions may not be injurious in themselves. I am

much indebted to Mr. H. H.  Howorth for having called my attention

to this subject, and for having given me information respecting it.

I have collected the following cases.



  * Bagehot, "Physics and Politics," Fortnightly Review, April 1,

1868, p. 455.



  When Tasmania was first colonised the natives were roughly estimated

by some at 7000 and by others at 20,000. Their number was soon greatly

reduced, chiefly by fighting with the English and with each other.

After the famous hunt by all the colonists, when the remaining natives

delivered themselves up to the government, they consisted only of

120 individuals,* who were in 1832 transported to Flinders Island.

This island, situated between Tasmania and Australia, is forty miles

long, and from twelve to eighteen miles broad: it seems healthy, and

the natives were well treated. Nevertheless, they suffered greatly

in health. In 1834 they consisted (Bonwick, p. 250) of forty-seven

adult males, forty-eight adult females, and sixteen children, or in

all of 111 souls. In 1835 only one hundred were left. As they

continued rapidly to decrease, and as they themselves thought that

they should not perish so quickly elsewhere, they were removed in 1847

to Oyster Cove in the southern part of Tasmania. They then consisted

(Dec. 20th, 1847) of fourteen men, twenty-two women and ten

children.*(2) But the change of site did no good. Disease and death

still pursued them, and in 1864 one man (who died in 1869), and

three elderly women alone survived. The infertility of the women is

even a more remarkable fact than the liability of all to ill-health

and death. At the time when only nine women were left at Oyster

Cove, they told Mr. Bonwick (p. 386), that only two had ever borne

children: and these two had together produced only three children!



  * All the statements here given are taken from The Last of the

Tasmanians, by J. Bonwick, 1870.

  *(2) This is the statement of the Governor of Tasmania, Sir W.

Denison, Varieties of Vice-Regal Life, 1870, vol. i., p. 67.



  With respect to the cause of this extraordinary state of things, Dr.

Story remarks that death followed the attempts to civilise the

natives. "If left to themselves to roam as they were wont and

undisturbed, they would have reared more children, and there would

have been less mortality." Another careful observer of the natives,

Mr. Davis, remarks, "The births have been few and the deaths numerous.

This may have been in a great measure owing to their change of

living and food; but more so to their banishment from the mainland

of Van Diemen's Land, and consequent depression of spirits"

(Bonwick, pp. 388, 390).

  Similar facts have been observed in two widely different parts of

Australia. The celebrated explorer, Mr. Gregory, told Mr. Bonwick,

that in Queensland "the want of reproduction was being already felt

with the blacks, even in the most recently settled parts, and that

decay would set in." Of thirteen aborigines from Shark's Bay who

visited Murchison River, twelve died of consumption within three

months.*



  * For these cases, see Bonwick's Daily Life of the Tasmanians, 1870,

p. 90: and The Last of the Tasmanians, 1870, p. 386.



  The decrease of the Maories of New Zealand has been carefully

investigated by Mr. Fenton, in an admirable report, from which all the

following statements, with one exception, are taken.* The decrease

in number since 1830 is admitted by every one, including the natives

themselves, and is still steadily progressing. Although it has

hitherto been found impossible to take an actual census of the

natives, their numbers were carefully estimated by residents in many

districts. The result seems trustworthy, and shows that during the

fourteen years, previous to 1858, the decrease was 19.42 per cent.

Some of the tribes, thus carefully examined, lived above a hundred

miles apart, some on the coast, some inland; and their means of

subsistence and habits differed to a certain extent (p. 28). The total

number in 1858 was believed to be 53,700, and in 1872, after a

second interval of fourteen years, another census was taken, and the

number is given as only 36,359, shewing a decrease of 32.29 per

cent!*(2) Mr. Fenton, after shewing in detail the insufficiency of the

various causes, usually assigned in explanation of this

extraordinary decrease, such as new diseases, the profligacy of the

women, drunkenness, wars, &c., concludes on weighty grounds that it

depends chiefly on the unproductiveness of the women, and on the

extraordinary mortality of the young children (pp. 31, 34). In proof

of this he shews (p. 33) that in 1844 there was one non-adult for

every 2.57 adults; whereas in 1858 there was only one non-adult for

every 3.27 adults. The mortality of the adults is also great. He

adduces as a further cause of the decrease the inequality of the

sexes; for fewer females are born than males. To this latter point,

depending perhaps on a widely distinct cause, I shall return in a

future chapter. Mr. Fenton contrasts with astonishment the decrease in

New Zealand with the increase in Ireland; countries not very

dissimilar in climate, and where the inhabitants now follow nearly

similar habits. The Maories themselves (p. 35) "attribute their

decadence, in some measure, to the introduction of new food and

clothing, and the attendant change of habits"; and it will be seen,

when we consider the influence of changed conditions on fertility,

that they are probably right. The diminution began between the years

1830 and 1840; and Mr. Fenton shews (p. 40) that about 1830, the art

of manufacturing putrid corn (maize), by long steeping in water, was

discovered and largely practised; and this proves that a change of

habits was beginning amongst the natives, even when New Zealand was

only thinly inhabited by Europeans. When I visited the Bay of

Islands in 1835, the dress and food of the inhabitants had already

been much modified: they raised potatoes, maize, and other

agricultural produce, and exchanged them for English manufactured

goods and tobacco.



  * Observations on the Aboriginal Inhabitants of New Zealand,

published by the Government, 1859.

  *(2) New Zealand, by Alex. Kennedy, 1873, p. 47.



  It is evident from many statements in the life of Bishop

Patteson,* that the Melanesians of the New Hebrides and neighbouring

archipelagoes, suffered to an extraordinary degree in health, and

perished in large numbers, when they were removed to New Zealand,

Norfolk Island, and other salubrious places, in order to be educated

as missionaries.



  * Life of J. C. Patteson, by C. M. Younge, 1874; see more especially

vol. i., p. 530.



  The decrease of the native population of the Sandwich Islands is

as notorious as that of New Zealand. It has been roughly estimated

by those best capable of judging, that when Cook discovered the

islands in 1779, the population amounted to about 300,000. According

to a loose census in 1823, the numbers then were 142,050. In 1832, and

at several subsequent periods, an accurate census was officially

taken, but I have been able to obtain only the following returns:



                Native Population          Annual rate of decrease

                                           per cent, assuming it to

              (Except during 1832 and      have been uniform between

              1836, when the few           the successive censuses;

              foreigners in the islands    these censuses being taken

  Year        were included.)              at irregular intervals.



  1832              130,313

                                                   4.46

  1836              108,579

                                                   2.47

  1853               71,019

                                                   0.81

  1860               67,084

                                                   2.18

  1866               58,765

                                                   2.17

  1872               51,531



  We here see that in the interval of forty years, between 1832 and

1872, the population has decreased no less than sixty-eight per

cent! This has been attributed by most writers to the profligacy of

the women, to former bloody wars, and to the severe labour imposed

on conquered tribes and to newly introduced diseases, which have

been on several occasions extremely destructive. No doubt these and

other such causes have been highly efficient, and may account for

the extraordinary rate of decrease between the years 1832 and 1836;

but the most potent of all the causes seems to be lessened

fertility. According to Dr. Ruschenberger of the U. S. Navy, who

visited these islands between 1835 and 1837, in one district of

Hawaii, only twenty-five men out of 1134, and in another district only

ten out of 637, had a family with as many as three children. Of eighty

married women, only thirty-nine had ever borne children; and "the

official report gives an average of half a child to each married

couple in the whole island." This is almost exactly the same average

as with the Tasmanians at Oyster Cove. Jarves, who published his

History in 1843, says that "families who have three children are freed

from all taxes; those having more, are rewarded by gifts of land and

other encouragements." This unparalleled enactment by the government

well shews how infertile the race had become. The Rev. A. Bishop

stated in the Hawaiian Spectator in 1839, that a large proportion of

the children die at early ages, and Bishop Staley informs me that this

is still the case, just as in New Zealand. This has been attributed to

the neglect of the children by the women, but it is probably in

large part due to innate weakness of constitution in the children,

in relation to the lessened fertility of their parents. There is,

moreover, a further resemblance to the case of New Zealand, in the

fact that there is a large excess of male over female births: the

census of 1872 gives 31,650 males to 25,247 females of all ages,

that is 125.36 males for every 100 females; whereas in all civilised

countries the females exceed the males. No doubt the profligacy of the

women may in part account for their small fertility; but their changed

habits of life is a much more probable cause, and which will at the

same time account for the increased mortality, especially of the

children. The islands were visited by Cook in 1779, Vancouver in 1794,

and often subsequently by whalers. In 1819 missionaries arrived, and

found that idolatry had been already abolished and other changes

effected by the king. After this period there was a rapid change in

almost all the habits of life of the natives, and they soon became

"the most civilised of the Pacific Islanders." One of my informants,

Mr. Coan, who was born on the islands, remarks that the natives have

undergone a greater change in their habits of life in the course of

fifty years than Englishmen during a thousand years. From

information received from Bishop Staley, it does not appear that the

poorer classes have ever much changed their diet, although many new

kinds of fruit have been introduced, and the sugar-cane is in

universal use. Owing, however, to their passion for imitating

Europeans, they altered their manner of dressing at an early period,

and the use of alcoholic drinks became very general. Although these

changes appear inconsiderable, I can well believe, from what is

known with respect to animals, that they might suffice to lessen the

fertility of the natives.*



  * The foregoing statements are taken chiefly from the following

works: Jarves' History of the Hawaiian Islands, 1843, pp. 400-407.

Cheever, Life in the Sandwich Islands, 1851, p. 277. Ruschenberger

is quoted by Bonwick, Last of the Tasmanians, 1870, p. 378. Bishop

is quoted by Sir E. Belcher, Voyage Round the World, 1843, vol. i., p.

272. I owe the census of the several years to the kindness of Mr.

Coan, at the request of Dr. Youmans of New York; and in most cases I

have compared the Youmans figures with those given in several of the

above-named works. I have omitted the census for 1850, as I have

seen two widely different numbers given.



  Lastly, Mr. Macnamara states* that the low and degraded

inhabitants of the Andaman Islands, on the eastern side of the Gulf of

Bengal, are "eminently susceptible to any change of climate: in

fact, take them away from their island homes, and they are almost

certain to die, and that independently of diet or extraneous

influences." He further states that the inhabitants of the Valley of

Nepal, which is extremely hot in summer, and also the various

hill-tribes of India, suffer from dysentery and fever when on the

plains; and they die if they attempt to pass the whole year there.



  * The Indian Medical Gazette, Nov. 1, 1871, p. 240.



  We thus see that many of the wilder races of man are apt to suffer

much in health when subjected to changed conditions or habits of life,

and not exclusively from being transported to a new climate. Mere

alterations in habits, which do not appear injurious in themselves,

seem to have this same effect; and in several cases the children are

particularly liable to suffer. It has often been said, as Mr.

Macnamara remarks, that man can resist with impunity the greatest

diversities of climate and other changes; but this is true only of the

civilised races. Man in his wild condition seems to be in this respect

almost as susceptible as his nearest allies, the anthropoid apes,

which have never yet survived long, when removed from their native

country.

  Lessened fertility from changed conditions, as in the case of the

Tasmanians, Maories, Sandwich Islanders, and apparently the

Australians, is still more interesting than their liability to

ill-health and death; for even a slight degree of infertility,

combined with those other causes which tend to check the increase of

every population, would sooner or later lead to extinction. The

diminution of fertility may be explained in some cases by the

profligacy of the women (as until lately with the Tahitians), but

Mr. Fenton has shewn that this explanation by no means suffices with

the New Zealanders, nor does it with the Tasmanians.

  In the paper above quoted, Mr. Macnamara gives reasons for believing

that the inhabitants of districts subject to malaria are apt to be

sterile; but this cannot apply in several of the above cases. Some

writers have suggested that the aborigines of islands have suffered in

fertility and health from long continued interbreeding; but in the

above cases infertility has coincided too closely with the arrival

of Europeans for us to admit this explanation. Nor have we at

present any reason to believe that man is highly sensitive to the evil

effects of inter-breeding, especially in areas so large as New

Zealand, and the Sandwich archipelago with its diversified stations.

On the contrary, it is known that the present inhabitants of Norfolk

Island are nearly all cousins or near relations, as are the Todas in

India, and the inhabitants of some of the Western Islands of Scotland;

and yet they seem not to have suffered in fertility.*



  * On the close relationship of the Norfolk Islanders, Sir W.

Denison, Varieties of Vice-Regal Life: vol. i., 1870, p. 410. For

the Todas, see Col. Marshall's work 1873, p. 110. For the Western

Islands of Scotland, Dr. Mitchell, Edinburgh Medical Journal, March to

June, 1865.



  A much more probable view is suggested by the analogy of the lower

animals. The reproductive system can be shewn to be susceptible to

an extraordinary degree (though why we know not) to changed conditions

of life; and this susceptibility leads both to beneficial and to

evil results. A large collection of facts on this subject is given

in chap. xviii. of vol. ii. of my Variation of Animals and Plants

under Domestication, I can here give only the briefest abstract; and

every one interested in the subject may consult the above work. Very

slight changes increase the health, vigour, and fertility of most or

all organic beings, whilst other changes are known to render a large

number of animals sterile. One of the most familiar cases, is that

of tamed elephants not breeding in India; though they often breed in

Ava, where the females are allowed to roam about the forests to some

extent, and are thus placed under more natural conditions. The case of

various American monkeys, both sexes of which have been kept for

many years together in their own countries, and yet have very rarely

or never bred, is a more apposite instance, because of their

relationship to man. It is remarkable how slight a change in the

conditions often induces sterility in a wild animal when captured; and

this is the more strange as all our domesticated animals have become

more fertile than they were in a state of nature; and some of them can

resist the most unnatural conditions with undiminished fertility.*

Certain groups of animals are much more liable than others to be

affected by captivity; and generally all the species of the same group

are affected in the same manner. But sometimes a single species in a

group is rendered sterile, whilst the others are not so; on the

other hand, a single species may retain its fertility whilst most of

the others fail to breed. The males and females of some species when

confined, or when allowed to live almost, but not quite free, in their

native country, never unite; others thus circumstanced frequently

unite but never produce offspring; others again produce some

offspring, but fewer than in a state of nature; and as bearing on

the above cases of man, it is important to remark that the young are

apt to be weak and sickly, or malformed, and to perish at an early

age.



  * For the evidence on this head, see Variation of Animals, &c., vol.

ii., p. 111.



  Seeing how general is this law of the susceptibility of the

reproductive system to changed conditions of life, and that it holds

good with our nearest allies, the Quadrumana, I can hardly doubt

that it applies to man in his primeval state. Hence if savages of

any race are induced suddenly to change their habits of life, they

become more or less sterile, and their young offspring suffer in

health, in the same manner and from the same cause, as do the elephant

and hunting-leopard in India, many monkeys in America, and a host of

animals of all kinds, on removal from their natural conditions.

  We can see why it is that aborigines, who have long inhabited

islands, and who must have been long exposed to nearly uniform

conditions, should be specially affected by any change in their

habits, as seems to be the case. Civilised races can certainly

resist changes of all kinds far better than savages; and in this

respect they resemble domesticated animals, for though the latter

sometimes suffer in health (for instance European dogs in India),

yet they are rarely rendered sterile, though a few such instances have

been recorded.* The immunity of civilised races and domesticated

animals is probably due to their having been subjected to a greater

extent, and therefore having grown somewhat more accustomed, to

diversified or varying conditions, than the majority of wild

animals; and to their having formerly immigrated or been carried

from country to country, and to different families or subraces

having inter-crossed. It appears that a cross with civilised races

at once gives to an aboriginal race an immunity from the evil

consequences of changed conditions. Thus the crossed offspring from

the Tahitians and English, when settled in Pitcairn Island,

increased so rapidly that the island was soon overstocked; and in June

1856 they were removed to Norfolk Island. They then consisted of 60

married persons and 134 children, making a total of 194. Here they

likewise increased so rapidly, that although sixteen of them

returned to Pitcairn Island in 1859, they numbered in January 1868,

300 souls; the males and females being in exactly equal numbers.

What a contrast does this case present with that of the Tasmanians;

the Norfolk Islanders increased in only twelve and a half years from

194 to 300; whereas the Tasmanians decreased during fifteen years from

120 to 46, of which latter number only ten were children.*(2)



  * Variation of Animals, &c., vol. ii., p. 16.

  *(2) These details are taken from The Mutineers of the Bounty, by

Lady Belcher, 1870; and from Pitcairn Island, ordered to be printed by

the House of Commons, May 29, 1863. The following statements about the

Sandwich Islanders are from the Honolulu Gazette, and from Mr. Coan.



  So again in the interval between the census of 1866 and 1872 the

natives of full blood in the Sandwich Islands decreased by 8081,

whilst the half-castes, who are believed to be healthier, increased by

847; but I do not know whether the latter number includes the

offspring from the half-castes, or only the half-castes of the first

generation.

  The cases which I have here given all relate to aborigines, who have

been subjected to new conditions as the result of the immigration of

civilised men. But sterility and ill-health would probably follow,

if savages were compelled by any cause, such as the inroad of a

conquering tribe, to desert their homes and to change their habits. It

is an interesting circumstance that the chief check to wild animals

becoming domesticated, which implies the power of their breeding

freely when first captured, and one chief check to wild men, when

brought into contact with civilisation, surviving to form a

civilised race, is the same, namely, sterility from changed conditions

of life.

  Finally, although the gradual decrease and ultimate extinction of

the races of man is a highly complex problem, depending on many causes

which differ in different places and at different times; it is the

same problem as that presented by the extinction of one of the

higher animals- of the fossil horse, for instance, which disappeared

from South America, soon afterwards to be replaced, within the same

districts, by countless troups of the Spanish horse. The New Zealander

seems conscious of this parallelism, for he compares his future fate

with that of the native rat now almost exterminated by the European

rat. Though the difficulty is great to our imagination, and really

great, if we wish to ascertain the precise causes and their manner

of action, it ought not to be so to our reason, as long as we keep

steadily in mind that the increase of each species and each race is

constantly checked in various ways; so that if any new check, even a

slight one, be superadded, the race will surely decrease in number;

and decreasing numbers will sooner or later lead to extinction; the

end, in most cases, being promptly determined by the inroads of

conquering tribes.

  On the Formation of the Races of Man.- In some cases the crossing of

distinct races has led to the formation of a new race. The singular

fact that the Europeans and Hindoos, who belong to the same Aryan

stock, and speak a language fundamentally the same, differ widely in

appearance, whilst Europeans differ but little from Jews, who belong

to the Semitic stock, and speak quite another language, has been

accounted for by Broca,* through certain Aryan branches having been

largely crossed by indigenous tribes during their wide diffusion. When

two races in close contact cross, the first result is a

heterogeneous mixture: thus Mr. Hunter, in describing the Santali orhill-tribes of India, says that hundreds of imperceptible gradations

may be traced "from the black, squat tribes of the mountains to the

tall olive-coloured Brahman, with his intellectual brow, calm eyes,

and high but narrow head"; so that it is necessary in courts of

justice to ask the witnesses whether they are Santalis or Hindoos.*(2)

Whether a heterogeneous people, such as the inhabitants of some of the

Polynesian islands, formed by the crossing of two distinct races, with

few or no pure members left, would ever become homogeneous, is not

known from direct evidence. But as with our domesticated animals, a

cross-breed can certainly be fixed and made uniform by careful

selection*(3) in the course of a few generations, we may infer that

the free inter-crossing of a heterogeneous mixture during a long

descent would supply the place of selection, and overcome any tendency

to reversion; so that the crossed race would ultimately become

homogeneous, though it might not partake in an equal degree of the

characters of the two parent-races.



  * "On Anthropology," translation, Anthropological Review, Jan.,

1868, p. 38.

  *(2) The Animals of Rural Bengal, 1868, p. 134.

  *(3) The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication vol.

ii., p. 95.



  Of all the differences between the races of man, the colour of the

skin is the most conspicuous and one of the best marked. It was

formerly thought that differences of this kind could be accounted

for by long exposure to different climates; but Pallas first shewed

that this is not tenable, and he has since been followed by almost all

anthropologists.* This view has been rejected chiefly because the

distribution of the variously coloured races, most of whom have long

inhabited their present homes, does not coincide with corresponding

differences of climate. Some little weight may be given to such

cases as that of the Dutch families, who, as we hear on excellent

authority,*(2) have not undergone the least change of colour after

residing for three centuries in South Africa. An argument on the

same side may likewise be drawn from the uniform appearance in various

parts of the world of gipsies and Jews, though the uniformity of the

latter has been somewhat exaggerated.*(3) A very damp or a very dry

atmosphere has been supposed to be more influential in modifying the

colour of the skin than mere heat; but as D'Orbigny in South

America, and Livingstone in Africa, arrived at diametrically

opposite conclusions with respect to dampness and dryness, any

conclusion on this head must be considered as very doubtful.*(4)



  * Pallas, Act. Acad. St. Petersburg, 1780, part ii., p. 69. He was

followed by Rudolphi, in his Beitrage zur Anthropologie, 1812. An

excellent summary of the evidence is given by Godron, De l'Espece,

1859, vol. ii., p. 246, &c.

  *(2) Sir Andrew Smith, as quoted by Knox, Races of Man, 1850, p.

473.

  *(3) See De Quatrefages on this head, Revue des Cours Scientifiques,

Oct. 17, 1868, p. 731.

  *(4) Livingstone's Travels and Researches in S. Africa, 1857, pp.

338, 339. D'Orbigny, as quoted by Godron, De l'Espece, vol. ii., p.

266.



  Various facts, which I have given elsewhere, prove that the colour

of the skin and hair is sometimes correlated in a surprising manner

with a complete immunity from the action of certain vegetable poisons,

and from the attacks of certain parasites. Hence it occurred to me,

that negroes and other dark races might have acquired their dark tints

by the darker individuals escaping from the deadly influence of the

miasma of their native countries, during a long series of generations.

  I afterwards found that this same idea had long ago occurred to

Dr. Wells.* It has long been known that negroes, and even mulattoes

are almost completely exempt from the yellow fever, so destructive

in tropical America.*(2) They likewise escape to a large extent the

fatal intermittent fevers, that prevail along at least 2600 miles of

the shores of Africa, and which annually cause one-fifth of the

white settlers to die, and another fifth to return home invalided.*(3)

This immunity in the negro seems to be partly inherent, depending on

some unknown peculiarity of constitution, and partly the result of

acclimatisation. Pouchet*(4) states that the negro regiments recruited

near the Soudan, and borrowed from the Viceroy of Egypt for the

Mexican war, escaped the yellow fever almost equally with the

negroes originally brought from various parts of Africa and accustomed

to the climate of the West Indies. That acclimatisation plays a

part, is shewn by the many cases in which negroes have become somewhat

liable to tropical fevers, after having resided for some time in a

colder climate.*(5) The nature of the climate under which the white

races have long resided likewise has some influence on them; for

during the fearful epidemic of yellow fever in Demerara during 1837,

Dr. Blair found that the death-rate of the immigrants was proportional

to the latitude of the country whence they had come. With the negro

the immunity, as far as it is the result of acclimatisation, implies

exposure during a prodigious length of time; for the aborigines of

tropical America who have resided there from time immemorial, are

not exempt from yellow fever; and the Rev. H. B. Tristram states, that

there are districts in nothern Africa which the native inhabitants are

compelled annually to leave, though the negroes can remain with

safety.



  * See a paper read before the Royal Soc. in 1813, and published in

his Essays in 1818. I have given an account of Dr. Wells' views in the

"Historical Sketch" (p. 2) to my Origin of Species. Various cases of

colour correlated with constitutional peculiarities are given in my

Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol. ii., pp.

227, 335.

  *(2) See, for instance, Nott and Gliddon, Types of Mankind, p. 68.

  *(3) Major Tulloch in a paper read before the Statistical Society,

April 20, 1840, and given in the Athenaeum, 1840, p. 353.

  *(4) The Plurality of the Human Race (translat.), 1864, p. 60.

  *(5) Quartrefages, Unite de l'Espece Humaine, 1861, p. 205. Wartz,

Introduction to Anthropology, translat., vol. i., 1863, p. 124.

Livingstone gives analogous cases in his Travels.



  That the immunity of the negro is in any degree correlated with

the colour of his skin is a mere conjecture: it may be correlated with

some difference in his blood, nervous system, or other tissues.

Nevertheless, from the facts above alluded to, and from some

connection apparently existing between complexion and a tendency to

consumption, the conjecture seemed to me not improbable.

Consequently I endeavoured, with but little success,* to ascertain how

far it holds good. The late Dr. Daniell, who had long lived on the

west coast of Africa, told me that he did not believe in any such

relation. He was himself unusually fair, and had withstood the climate

in a wonderful manner. When he first arrived as a boy on the coast, an

old and experienced negro chief predicted from his appearance that

this would prove the case. Dr. Nicholson, of Antigua, after having

attended to this subject, writes to me that dark-coloured Europeans

escape the yellow fever more than those that are light-coloured. Mr.

J. M. Harris altogether denies that Europeans with dark hair withstand

a hot climate better than other men: on the contrary, experience has

taught him in making a selection of men for service on the coast of

Africa, to choose those with red hair.*(2) As far, therefore, as these

slight indications go, there seems no foundation for the hypothesis,

that blackness has resulted from the darker and darker individuals

having survived better during long exposure to fever-generating

miasma.



  * In the spring of 1862 I obtained permission from the

Director-General of the Medical department of the Army, to transmit to

the surgeons of the various regiments on foreign service a blank

table, with the following appended remarks, but I have received no

returns. "As several well-marked cases have been recorded with our

domestic animals of a relation between the colour of the dermal

appendages and the constitution; and it being notorious that there

is some limited degree of relation between the colour of the races

of man and the climate inhabited by them; the following

investigation seems worth consideration. Namely, whether there is

any relation in Europeans between the colour of their hair, and

their liability to the diseases of tropical countries. If the surgeons

of the several regiments, when stationed in unhealthy tropical

districts, would be so good as first to count, as a standard of

comparison, how many men, in the force whence the sick are drawn, have

dark and light-coloured hair, and hair of intermediate or doubtful

tints; and if a similar account were kept by the same medical

gentlemen, of all the men who suffered from malarious and yellow

fevers, or from dysentery, it would soon be apparent, after some

thousand cases had been tabulated, whether there exists any relation

between the colour of the hair and constitutional liability to

tropical diseases. Perhaps no such relation would be discovered, but

the investigation is well worth making. In case any positive result

were obtained, it might be of some practical use in selecting men

for any particular service. Theoretically the result would be of

high interest, as indicating one means by which a race of men

inhabiting from a remote period an unhealthy tropical climate, might

have become dark-coloured by the better preservation of dark-haired or

dark-complexioned individuals during a long succession of

generations."

  *(2) Anthropological Review, Jan., 1866, p. xxi. Dr. Sharpe also

says, with respect to India (Man a Special Creation, 1873, p. 118),

"that it has been noticed by some medical officers that Europeans with

light hair and florid complexions suffer less from diseases of

tropical countries than persons with dark hair and sallow complexions;

and, so far as I know, there appear to be good grounds for this

remark." On the other hand, Mr. Heddle, of Sierra Leone, "who has

had more clerks killed under him than any other man," by the climate

of the west African coast (W. Reade, African Sketch Book, vol. ii., p.

522), holds a directly opposite view, as does Capt. Burton.



  Dr. Sharpe remarks,* that a tropical sun, which burns and blisters a

white skin, does not injure a black one at all; and, as he adds,

this is not due to habit in the individual, for children only six or

eight months old are often carried about naked, and are not

affected. I have been assured by a medical man, that some years ago

during each summer, but not during the winter, his hands became marked

with light brown patches, like, although larger than freckles, and

that these patches were never affected by sun-burning, whilst the

white parts of his skin have on several occasions been much inflamed

and blistered. With the lower animals there is, also, a constitutional

difference in liability to the action of the sun between those parts

of the skin clothed with white hair and other parts.*(2) Whether the

saving of the skin from being thus burnt is of sufficient importance

to account for a dark tint having been gradually acquired by man

through natural selection, I am unable to judge. If it be so, we

should have to assume that the natives of tropical America have

lived there for a much shorter time than the Negroes in Africa or

the Papuans in the southern parts of the Malay archipelago, just as

the lighter-coloured Hindoos have resided in India for a shorter

time than the darker aborigines of the central and southern parts of

the peninsula.



  * Man a Special Creation, 1873, p. 119.

  *(2) Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol.

ii., pp. 336, 337.



  Although with our present knowledge we cannot account for the

differences of colour in the races of man, through any advantage

thus gained, or from the direct action of climate; yet we must not

quite ignore the latter agency, for there is good reason to believe

that some inherited effect is thus produced.*



  * See, for instance, Quatrefages (Revue des Cours Scientifiques,

Oct. 10, 1868, p. 724) on the effects of residence in Abyssinia and

Arabia, and other analogous cases. Dr. Rolle (Der Mensch, seine

Abstammung, &c., 1865, s. 99) states, on the authority of Khanikof,

that the greater number of German families settled in Georgia, have

acquired in the course of two generations dark hair and eyes. Mr. D.

Forbes informs me that the Quechuas in the Andes vary greatly in

colour, according to the position of the valleys inhabited by them.



  We have seen in the second chapter that the conditions of life

affect the development of the bodily frame in a direct manner, and

that the effects are transmitted. Thus, as is generally admitted,

the European settlers in the United States undergo a slight but

extraordinary rapid change of appearance. Their bodies and limbs

become elongated; and I hear from Col. Bernys that during the late war

in the United States, good evidence was afforded of this fact by the

ridiculous appearance presented by the German regiments, when

dressed in ready-made clothes manufactured for the American market,

and which were much too long for the men in every way. There is, also,

a considerable body of evidence shewing that in the Southern States

the house-slaves of the third generation present a markedly

different appearance from the field-slaves.*



  * Harlan, Medical Researches, p. 532. Quatrefages (Unite de l'Espece

Humaine, 1861, p. 128) has collected much evidence on this head.



  If, however, we look to the races of man as distributed over the

world, we must infer that their characteristic differences cannot be

accounted for by the direct action of different conditions of life,

even after exposure to them for an enormous period of time. The

Esquimaux live exclusively on animal food; they are clothed in thick

fur, and are exposed to intense cold and to prolonged darkness; yet

they do not differ in any extreme degree from the inhabitants of

southern China, who live entirely on vegetable food, and are exposed

almost naked to a hot, glaring climate. The unclothed Fuegians live on

the marine productions of their inhospitable shores; the Botocudos

of Brazil wander about the hot forests of the interior and live

chiefly on vegetable productions; yet these tribes resemble each other

so closely that the Fuegians on board the "Beagle" were mistaken by

some Brazilians for Botocudos. The Botocudos again, as well as the

other inhabitants of tropical America, are wholly different from the

Negroes who inhabit the opposite shores of the Atlantic, are exposed

to a nearly similar climate, and follow nearly the same habits of

life.

  Nor can the differences between the races of man be accounted for by

the inherited effects of the increased or decreased use of parts,

except to a quite insignificant degree. Men who habitually live in

canoes, may have their legs somewhat stunted; those who inhabit

lofty regions may have their chests enlarged; and those who constantly

use certain sense-organs may have the cavities in which they are

lodged somewhat increased in size, and their features consequently a

little modified. With civilized nations, the reduced size of the

jaws from lessened use- the habitual play of different muscles serving

to express different emotions- and the increased size of the brain

from greater intellectual activity, have together produced a

considerable effect on their general appearance when compared with

savages.* Increased bodily stature, without any corresponding increase

in the size of the brain, may (judging from the previously adduced

case of rabbits), have given to some races an elongated skull of the

dolichocephalic type.



  * See Prof. Schaaffhausen, translat., in Anthropological Review,

Oct., 1868, p. 429.



  Lastly, the little-understood principle of correlated development

has sometimes come into action, as in the case of great muscular

development and strongly projecting supra-orbital ridges. The colour

of the skin and hair are plainly correlated, as is the texture of

the hair with its colour in the Mandans of North America.* The

colour also of the skin, and the odour emitted by it, are likewise

in some manner connected. With the breeds of sheep the number of hairs

within a given space and the number of excretory pores are

related.*(2) If we may judge from the analogy of our domesticated

animals, many modifications of structure in man probably come under

this principle of correlated development.



  * Mr. Catlin states (N. American Indians, 3rd ed., 1842, vol. i., p.

49) that in the whole tribe of the Mandans, about one in ten or twelve

of the members, of all ages and both sexes, have bright silvery grey

hair, which is hereditary. Now this hair is as coarse and harsh as

that of a horse's mane, whilst the hair of other colours is fine and

soft.

  *(2) On the odour of the skin, Godron, De l'Espece, tom. ii., p.

217. On the pores of the skin, Dr. Wilckens, Die Aufgaben der

Landwirth. Zootechnik, 1869, s. 7.



  We have now seen that the external characteristic differences

between the races of man cannot be accounted for in a satisfactory

manner by the direct action of the conditions of life, nor by the

effects of the continued use of parts, nor through the principle of

correlation. We are therefore led to enquire whether slight individual

differences, to which man is eminently liable, may not have been

preserved and augmented during a long series of generations through

natural selection. But here we are at once met by the objection that

beneficial variations alone can be thus preserved; and as far as we

are enabled to judge, although always liable to err on this head, none

of the differences between the races of man are of any direct or

special service to him. The intellectual and moral or social faculties

must of course be excepted from this remark. The great variability

of all the external differences between the races of man, likewise

indicates that they cannot be of much importance; for if important,

they would long ago have been either fixed and preserved, or

eliminated. In this respect man resembles those forms, called by

naturalists protean or polymorphic, which have remained extremely

variable, owing, as it seems, to such variations being of an

indifferent nature, and to their having thus escaped the action of

natural selection.

  We have thus far been baffled in all our attempts to account for the

differences between the races of man; but there remains one

important agency, namely Sexual Selection, which appears to have acted

powerfully on man, as on many other animals. I do not intend to assert

that sexual selection will account for all the differences between the

races. An unexplained residuum is left, about which we can only say,

in our ignorance, that as individuals are continually born with, for

instance, heads a little rounder or narrower, and with noses a

little longer or shorter, such slight differences might become fixed

and uniform, if the unknown agencies which induced them were to act in

a more constant manner, aided by long-continued intercrossing. Such

variations come under the provisional class, alluded to in our

second chapter, which for want of a better term are often called

spontaneous. Nor do I pretend that the effects of sexual selection can

be indicated with scientific precision; but it can be shewn that it

would be an inexplicable fact if man had not been modified by this

agency, which appears to have acted powerfully on innumerable animals.

It can further be shewn that the differences between the races of man,

as in colour, hairiness, form of features, &c., are of a kind which

might have been expected to come under the influence of sexual

selection. But in order to treat this subject properly, I have found

it necessary to pass the whole animal kingdom in review. I have

therefore devoted to it the Second Part of this work. At the close I

shall return to man, and, after attempting to shew how far he has been

modified through sexual selection, will give a brief summary of the

chapters in this First Part.



  NOTE ON THE RESEMBLANCES AND DIFFERENCES IN THE STRUCTURE AND THE

DEVELOPMENT OF THE BRAIN IN MAN AND APES. BY PROFESSOR HUXLEY, F. R.

S.



  The controversy respecting the nature and the extent of the

differences in the structure of the brain in man and the apes, which

arose some fifteen years ago, has not yet come to an end, though the

subject matter of the dispute is, at present, totally different from

what it was formerly. It was originally asserted and re-asserted, with

singular pertinacity that the brain of all the apes, even the highest,

differs from that of man, in the absence of such conspicuous

structures as the posterior lobes of the cerebral hemispheres, with

the posterior cornu of the lateral ventricle and the hippocampus

minor, contained in those lobes, which are so obvious in man.

  But the truth that the structures in question are as well

developed in apes' as in human brains, or even better; and that it

is characteristic of all the primates (if we exclude the lemurs) to

have these parts well developed, stands at present on as secure a

basis as any proposition in comparative anatomy. Moreover, it is

admitted by every one of the long series of anatomists who, of late

years, have paid special attention to the arrangement of the

complicated sulci 0and gyri which appear upon the surface of the

cerebral hemispheres in man and the higher apes, that they are

disposed after the very same pattern in him, as in them. Every

principal gyrus and sulcus of a chimpanzee's brain is clearly

represented in that of a man, so that the terminology which applies to

the one answers for the other. On this point there is no difference of

opinion. Some years since, Professor Bischoff published a memoir* on

the cerebral convolutions of man and apes; and as the purpose of my

learned colleague was certainly not to diminish the value of the

differences between apes and men in this respect, I am glad to make

a citation from him.



  * "Die Grosshirnwindungen des Menschen"; Abhandlungen der K.

Bayerischen Akademie, B. x., 1868.



  "That the apes, and especially the orang, chimpanzee and gorilla,

come very close to man in their organisation, much nearer than to

any other animal, is a well known fact, disputed by nobody. Looking at

the matter from the point of view of organisation alone, no one

probably would ever have disputed the view of Linnaeus, that man

should be placed, merely as a peculiar species, at the head of the

mammalia and of those apes. Both shew, in all their organs, so close

an affinity, that the most exact anatomical investigation is needed in

order to demonstrate those differences which really exist. So it is

with the brains. The brains of man, the orang, the chimpanzee, the

gorilla, in spite of all the important differences which they present,

come very close to one another" (loc. cit., p. 101).

  There remains, then, no dispute as to the resemblance in fundamental

characters, between the ape's brain and man's: nor any as to the

wonderfully close similarity between the chimpanzee, orang and man, in

even the details of the arrangement of the gyri and sulci of the

cerebral hemispheres. Nor, turning to the differences between the

brains of the highest apes and that of man, is there any serious

question as to the nature and extent of these differences. It is

admitted that the man's cerebral hemispheres are absolutely and

relatively larger than those of the orang and chimpanzee; that his

frontal lobes are less excavated by the upward protrusion of the

roof of the orbits; that his gyri and sulci are, as a rule, less

symmetrically disposed, and present a greater number of secondary

plications. And it is admitted that, as a rule, in man, the

temporo-occipital or "external perpendicular" fissure, which is

usually so strongly marked a feature of the ape's brain is but faintly

marked. But it is also clear, that none of these differences

constitutes a sharp demarcation between the man's and the ape's brain.

In respect to the external perpendicular fissure of Gratiolet, in

the human brain for instance, Professor Turner remarks:*



  * Convolutions of the Human Cerebrum Topographically Considered,

1866, p. 12.



  "In some brains it appears simply as an indentation of the margin of

the hemisphere, but, in others, it tends for some distance more or

less transversely outwards. I saw it in the right hemisphere of a

female brain pass more than two inches outwards; and on another

specimen, also the right hemisphere, it proceeded for four-tenths of

an inch outwards, and then extended downwards, as far as the lower

margin of the outer surface of the hemisphere. The imperfect

definition of this fissure in the majority of human brains, as

compared with its remarkable distinctness in the brain of most

Quadrumana, is owing to the presence, in the former, of certain

superficial, well marked, secondary convolutions which bridge it

over and connect the parietal with the occipital lobe. The closer

the first of these bridging gyri lies to the longitudinal fissure, the

shorter is the external parieto-occipital fissure" (loc. cit., p. 12).

  The obliteration of the external perpendicular fissure of Gratiolet,

therefore, is not a constant character of the human brain. On the

other hand, its full development is not a constant character of the

higher ape's brain. For, in the chimpanzee, the more or less extensive

obliteration of the external perpendicular sulcus by "bridging

convolutions," on one side or the other, has been noted over and

over again by Prof. Rolleston, Mr. Marshall, M. Broca and Professor

Turner. At the conclusion of a special paper on this subject the

latter writes:*



  * "Notes more especially on the bridging convolutions in the Brain

of the Chimpanzee," Proceedings of the Royal Society of Edinburgh,

1865-6.



 "The three specimens of the brain of a Chimpanzee," just described,

prove that the generalisation which Gratiolet has attempted to draw of

the complete absence of the first connecting convolution and the

concealment of the second, as essentially characteristic features in

the brain of this animal, is by no means universally applicable. In

only one specimen did the brain, in these particulars, follow the

law which Gratiolet has expressed. As regards the presence of the

superior bridging convolution, I am inclined to think that it has

existed in one hemisphere, at least, in a majority of the brains of

this animal which have, up to this time, been figured or described.

The superficial position of the second bridging convolution is

evidently less frequent, and has as yet, I believe, only been seen

in the brain (A) recorded in this communication. The asymmetrical

arrangement in the convolutions of the two hemispheres, which previous

observers have referred to in their descriptions, is also well

illustrated in these specimens" (pp. 8, 9).

  Even were the presence of the temporo-occipital, or external

perpendicular, sulcus, a mark of distinction between the higher apes

and man, the value of such a distinctive character would be rendered

very doubtful by the structure of the brain in the platyrhine apes. In

fact, while the temporo-occipital is one of the most constant of sulci

in the catarhine, or Old World, apes, it is never very strongly

developed in the New World apes; it is absent in the smaller

platyrhine; rudimentary in Pithecia;* and more or less obliterated

by bridging convolutions in Ateles.



  * FIower, "On the Anatomy of Pithecia Monachus," Proceedings of

the Zoological Society, 1862.



  A character which is thus variable within the limits of a single

group can have no great taxonomic value.

  It is further established, that the degree of asymmetry of the

convolution of the two sides in the human brain is subject to much

individual variation; and that, in those individuals of the bushman

race who have been examined, the gyri and sulci of the two hemispheres

are considerably less complicated and more symmetrical than in theEuropean brain, while, in some individuals of the chimpanzee, their

complexity and asymmetry become notable. This is particularly the case

in the brain of a young male chimpanzee figured by M. Broca.

(L'ordre des Primates, p. 165, fig. 11.)

  Again, as respects the question of absolute size, it is

established that the difference between the largest and the smallest

healthy human brain is greater than the difference between the

smallest healthy human brain and the largest chimpanzee's or orang's

brain.

  Moreover, there is one circumstance in which the orang's and

chimpanzee's brains resemble man's, but in which they differ from

the lower apes, and that is the presence of two corpora candicantia-

the Cynomorpha having but one.

  In view of these facts I do not hesitate in this year 1874, to

repeat and insist upon the proposition which I enunciated in 1863:*



  * Man's Place in Nature, p. 102.



  "So far as cerebral structure goes, therefore, it is clear that

man differs less from the chimpanzee or the orang, than these do

even from the monkeys, and that the difference between the brain of

the chimpanzee and of man is almost insignificant when compared with

that between the chimpanzee brain and that of a lemur."

  In the paper to which I have referred, Professor Bischoff does not

deny the second part of this statement, but he first makes the

irrelevant remark that it is not wonderful if the brains of an orang

and a lemur are very different; and secondly, goes on to assert

that, "If we successively compare the brain of a man with that of an

orang; the brain of this with that of a chimpanzee; of this with

that of a gorilla, and so on of a Hylobates, Semnopithecus,

Cynocephalus, Cercopithecus, Macacus, Cebus, Callithrix, Lemur,

Stenops, Hapale, we shall not meet with a greater, or even as great

a break in the degree of development of the convolutions, as we find

between the brain of a man and that of an orang or chimpanzee."

  To which I reply, firstly, that whether this assertion be true or

false, it has nothing whatever to do with the proposition enunciated

in Man's Place in Nature, which refers not to the development of the

convolutions alone, but to the structure of the whole brain. If

Professor Bischoff had taken the trouble to refer to p. 96 of the work

he criticises, in fact, he would have found the following passage:

"And it is a remarkable circumstance that though, so far as our

present knowledge extends, there is one true structural break in the

series of forms of simian brains, this hiatus does not lie between man

and the manlike apes, but between the lower and the lowest simians, or

in other words, between the Old and New World apes and monkeys and the

lemurs. Every lemur which has yet been examined, in fact, has its

cerebellum partially visible from above; and its posterior lobe,

with the contained posterior cornu and hippocampus minor, more or less

rudimentary. Every marmoset, American monkey, Old World monkey, baboon

or manlike ape, on the contrary, has its cerebellum entirely hidden,

posteriorly, by the cerebral lobes, and possesses a large posterior

cornu with a well-developed hippocampus minor."

  This statement was a strictly accurate account of what was known

when it was made; and it does not appear to me to be more than

apparently weakened by the subsequent discovery of the relatively

small development of the posterior lobes in the siamang and in the

howling monkey. Notwithstanding the exceptional brevity of the

posterior lobes in these two species, no one will pretend that their

brains, in the slightest degree, approach those of the lemurs. And if,

instead of putting Hapale out of its natural place, as Professor

Bischoff most unaccountably does, we write the series of animals he

has chosen to mention as follows: Homo, Pithecus, Troglodytes,

Hylobates, Semnopithecus, Cynocephalus, Cereopithecus, Macacus, Cebus,

Callithrix, Hapale, Lemur, Stenops, I venture to reaffirm that the

great break in this series lies between Hapale and Lemur, and that

this break is considerably greater than that between any other two

terms of that series. Professor Bischoff ignores the fact that long

before he wrote, Gratiolet had suggested the separation of the

lemurs from the other primates on the very ground of the difference in

their cerebral characters; and that Professor Flower had made the

following observations in the course of his description of the brain

of the Javan loris:*



  * Transactions of the Zoological Society, vol. v., 1862.



  "And it is especially remarkable that, in the development of the

posterior lobes, there is no approximation to the lemurine, short

hemisphered brain, in those monkeys which are commonly supposed to

approach this family in other respects, viz., the lower members of the

platyrhine group."

  So far as the structure of the adult brain is concerned, then, the

very considerable additions to our knowledge, which have been made

by the researches of so many investigators, during the past ten years,

fully justify the statement which I made in 1863. But it has been

said, that, admitting the similarity between the adult brains of man

and apes, they are nevertheless, in reality, widely different, because

they exhibit fundamental differences in the mode of their development.

No one would be more ready than I to admit the force of this argument,

if such fundamental differences of development really exist. But I

deny that they do exist. On the contrary, there is a fundamental

agreement in the development of the brain in men and apes.

  Gratiolet originated the statement that there is a fundamental

difference in the development of the brains of apes and that of man-

consisting in this; that, in the apes, the sulci which first make

their appearance are situated on the posterior region of the

cerebral hemispheres, while, in the human foetus, the sulci first

become visible on the frontal lobes.*



  * "Chez tous les singes, les plis posterieurs se developpent les

premiers; les plis anterieurs se developpent plus tard, aussi la

vertebre occipitale et la parietale sont-elles relativement

tres-grandes chez le foetus. L'Homme presente une exception

remarquable quant a l'epoque de l'apparition des plis frontaux, qui

sont les premiers indiques; mais le developpement general du lobe

frontal, envisage seulement par rapport a son volume, suit les memes

lois que dans les singes"; Gratiolet, Memoire sur les plis cerebres de

l'Homme et des Primateaux, p. 39, tab. iv, fig. 3.



  This general statement is based upon two observations, the one of

a gibbon almost ready to be born, in which the posterior gyri were

"well developed," while those of the frontal lobes were "hardly

indicated"* (loc. cit., p. 39), and the other of a human foetus at the

22nd or 23rd week of utero-gestation, in which Gratiolet notes that

the insula was uncovered, but that nevertheless "des incisures

sement de lobe anterieur, une scissure peu profonde indique la

separation du lobe occipital, tres-reduit, d'ailleurs des cette

epoque. Le reste de la surface cerebrale est encore absolument lisse."



  * Gratiolet's words are (loc. cit., p. 39): "Dans le foetus dont

il s'agit les plis cerebraux posterieurs sont bien developpes,

tandis que les plis du lobe frontal sont a peine indiques." The

figure, however (pl. iv, fig. 3), shews the fissure of Rolando, and

one of the frontal sulci plainly enough. Nevertheless, M. Alix, in his

"Notice sur les travaux anthropologiques de Gratiolet" (Mem. de la

Societe d'Anthropologie de Paris, 1868, page 32), writes thus:

"Gratiolet a eu entre les mains le cerveau d'un foetus de Gibbon,

singe eminemment superieur, et tellement rapproche de l'orang, que des

naturalistes tres-competents l'ont range parmi les anthropoides. M.

Huxley, par exemple, n'hesite pas sur ce point. Eh bien, c'est sur

le cerveau d'un foetus de Gibbon que Gratiolet a vu les

circonvolutions du lobe temporo-sphenoidal deja developpees

lorsqu'il n'existent pas encore de plis sur le lobe frontal. Il

etait donc bien autorise a dire que, chez l'homme les

circonvolutions apparaissent d' a en w, tandis que chez les singes

elles se developpent d'w en a."



  Three views of this brain are given in plate II, figs. 1, 2, 3, of

the work cited, shewing the upper, lateral and inferior views of the

hemispheres, but not the inner view. It is worthy of note that the

figure by no means bears out Gratiolet's description, inasmuch as

the fissure (antero-temporal) on the posterior half of the face of the

hemisphere is more marked than any of those vaguely indicated in the

anterior half. If the figure is correct, it in no way justifies

Gratiolet's conclusion: "Il y a donc entre ces cerveaux [those of a

Callithrix and of a gibbon] et celui du foetus humain une difference

fondamental. Chez celui-ci, longtemps avant que les plis temporaux

apparaissent, les plis frontaux, essayent d'exister."

  Since Gratiolet's time, however, the development of the gyri and

sulci of the brain has been made the subject of renewed

investigation by Schmidt, Bischoff, Pansch,* and more particularly

by Ecker,*(2) whose work is not only the latest, but by far the most

complete, memoir on the subject.



  * Uber die typische Anordnung der Furchen und Windungen auf den

Grosshirn-Hemispharen des Menschen und der Affen," Archiv fur

Anthropologie, iii., 1868

  *(2) "Zur Entwicklungsgeschichte der Furchen und Windungen der

Grosshirn-Hemispharen im Foetus des Menschen." Archiv fur

Anthropologie, iii., 1868.



  The final results of their inquiries may be summed up as follows:-

  1. In the human foetus, the sylvian fissure is formed in the

course of the third month of utero-gestation. In this, and in the

fourth month, the cerebral hemispheres are smooth and rounded (with

the exception of the sylvian depression), and they project backwards

far beyond the cerebellum.

  2. The sulci, properly so called, begin to appear in the interval

between the end of the fourth and the beginning of the sixth month

of foetal life, but Ecker is careful to point out that, not only the

time, but the order, of their appearance is subject to considerable

individual variation. In no case, however, are either the frontal or

the temporal sulci the earliest.

  The first which appears, in fact, lies on the inner face of the

hemisphere (whence doubtless Gratiolet, who does not seem to have

examined that face in his foetus, overlooked it), and is either the

internal perpendicular (occipito-parietal), or the calcarine sulcus,

these two being close together and eventually running into one

another. As a rule the occipito-parietal is the earlier of the two.

  3. At the latter part of this period, another sulcus, the

"posterio-parietal," or "Fissure of Rolando" is developed, and it is

followed, in the course of the sixth month, by the other principal

sulci of the frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital lobes. There

is, however, no clear evidence that one of these constantly appears

before the other; and it is remarkable that, in the brain at the

period described and figured by Ecker (loc. cit., pp. 212-213 tab. II,

figs. 1, 2, 3, 4), the antero-temporal sulcus (scissure parallele)

so characteristic of the ape's brain, is as well, if not better

developed than the fissure of Rolando, and is much more marked than

the proper frontal sulci.

  Taking the facts as they now stand, it appears to me that the

order of the appearance of the sulci and gyri in the foetal human

brain is in perfect harmony with the general doctrine of evolution,

and with the view that man has been evolved from some ape-like form;

though there can be no doubt that that form was, in many respects,

different from any member of the primates now living.

  Von Baer taught us, half a century ago, that, in the course of their

development, allied animals put on at first, the characters of the

greater groups to which they belong, and, by degrees, assume those

which restrict them within the limits of their family, genus, and

species; and he proved, at the same time, that no developmental

stage of a higher animal is precisely similar to the adult condition

of any lower animal. It is quite correct to say that a frog passes

through the condition of a fish, inasmuch as at one period of its life

the tadpole has all the characters of a fish, and if it went no

further, would have to be grouped among fishes. But it is equally true

that a tadpole is very different from any known fish.

  In like manner, the brain of a human foetus, at the fifth month, may

correctly be said to be, not only the brain of an ape, but that of

an arctopithecine or marmoset-like ape; for its hemispheres, with

their great posterior lobster, and with no sulci but the sylvian and

the calcarine, present the characteristics found only in the group

of the arctopithecine primates. But it is equally true, as Gratiolet

remarks, that, in its widely open sylvian fissure, it differs from the

brain of any actual marmoset. No doubt it would be much more similar

to the brain of an advanced foetus of a marmoset. But we know

nothing whatever of the development of the brain in the marmosets.

In the Platyrhini proper, the only observation with which I am

acquainted is due to Pansch, who found in the brain of a foetal

Cebus apella, in addition to the sylvian fissure and the deep

calcarine fissure, only a very shallow antero-temporal fissure

(scissure parallele of Gratiolet).

  Now this fact, taken together with the circumstance that the

antero-temporal sulcus is present in such Platyrhini as the Saimiri,

which present mere traces of sulci on the anterior half of the

exterior of the cerebral hemispheres, or none at all, undoubtedly,

so far as it goes, affords fair evidence in favour of Gratiolet's

hypothesis, that the posterior sulci appear before the anterior, in

the brains of the Platyrhini. But, it by no means follows, that the

rule which may hold good for the Platyrhini extends to the

Catarhini. We have no information whatever respecting the

development of the brain in the Cynomorphia; and, as regards the

Anthropomorpha, nothing but the account of the brain of the gibbon

near birth, already referred to. At the present moment there is not

a shadow of evidence to show that the sulci of a chimpanzee's, or

orang's, brain do not appear in the same order as a man's.

  Gratiolet opens his preface with the aphorism: "Il est dangereux

dans les sciences de conclure trop vite." I fear he must have

forgotten this sound maxim by the time he had reached the discussion

of the differences between men and apes, in the body of his work. No

doubt, the excellent author of one of the most remarkable

contributions to the just understanding of the mammalian brain which

has ever been made, would have been the first to admit the

insufficiency of his data had he lived to profit by the advance of

inquiry. The misfortune is that his conclusions have been employed

by persons incompetent to appreciate their foundation, as arguments in

favour of obscurantism.*



  * For example, M. l'Abbe Lecomte in his terrible pamphlet, Le

Darwinisme et l'origine de l'homme, 1873.



  But it is important to remark that, whether Gratiolet was right or

wrong in his hypothesis respecting the relative order of appearance of

the temporal and frontal sulci, the fact remains; that. before

either temporal or frontal sulci, appear, the foetal brain of man

presents characters which are found only in the lowest group of the

primates (leaving out the lemurs); and that this is exactly what we

should expect to be the case, if man has resulted from the gradual

modification of the same form as that from which the other primates

have sprung.




Search the Secular Web | Bookstore | What's new? | Add an URL | Send Feedback | Disclaimer

[Internet Infidels] [Freethought Ring] [Church-State Ring]  [Free Speech Online]

Copyright © Internet Infidels 1995-1997. All rights reserved.

Last updated: Friday, 30-Jan-98 10:45:16 MST