Permanent
E-mail Addresses Freethought Audio
Tapes Products for Freethinkers

Library: Historical Documents: Charles Darwin: Descent of Man: Chapter 8


This file has been made available by the Bank Of Wisdom.

Descent of Man [ 1871 ]

Charles Darwin [ 1809 - 1882 ]

 

Part Two - Sexual Selection

Chapter VIII - Principles of Sexual Selection




  WITH animals which have their sexes separated, the males necessarily

differ from the females in their organs of reproduction; and these are

the primary sexual characters. But the sexes often differ in what

Hunter has called secondary sexual characters, which are not

directly connected with the act of reproduction; for instance, the

male possesses certain organs of sense or locomotion, of which the

female is quite destitute, or has them more highly-developed, in order

that he may readily find or reach her; or again the male has special

organs of prehension for holding her securely. These latter organs, of

infinitely diversified kinds, graduate into those which are commonly

ranked as primary, and in some cases can hardly be distinguished

from them; we see instances of this in the complex appendages at the

apex of the abdomen in male insects. Unless indeed we confine the term

"primary" to the reproductive glands, it is scarcely possible to

decide which ought to be called primary and which secondary.

  The female often differs from the male in having organs for the

nourishment or protection of her young, such as the mammary glands

of mammals, and the abdominal sacks of the marsupials. In some few

cases also the male possesses similar organs, which are wanting in the

female, such as the receptacles for the ova in certain male fishes,

and those temporarily developed in certain male frogs. The females

of most bees are provided with a special apparatus for collecting

and carrying pollen, and their ovipositor is modified into a sting for

the defense of the larvae and the community. Many similar cases

could be given, but they do not here concern us. There are, however,

other sexual differences quite unconnected with the primary

reproductive organs, and it is with these that we are more

especially concerned such as the greater size, strength, and pugnacity

of the male, his weapons of offence or means of defence against

rivals, his gaudy colouring and various ornaments, his power of

song, and other such characters.

  Besides the primary and secondary sexual differences, such as the

foregoing, the males and females of some animals differ in

structures related to different habits of life, and not at all, or

only indirectly, to the reproductive functions. Thus the females of

certain flies (Culicidae and Tabanidae) are blood-suckers, whilst

the males, living on flowers, have mouths destitute of mandibles.* The

males of certain moths and of some crustaceans (e. g. Tanais) have

imperfect, closed mouths, and cannot feed. The complemental males of

certain cirripedes live like epiphytic plants either on the female

or the hermaphrodite form, and are destitute of a mouth and of

prehensile limbs. In these cases it is the male which has been

modified, and has lost certain important organs, which the females

possess. In other cases it is the female which has lost such parts;

for instance, the female glow-worm is destitute of wings, as also

are many female moths, some of which never leave their cocoons. Many

female parasitic crustaceans have lost their natatory legs. In some

weevil-beetles (Curculionidae) there is a great difference between the

male and female in the length of the rostrum or snout;*(2) but the

meaning of this and of many analogous differences, is not at all

understood. Differences of structure between the two sexes in relation

to different habits of life are generally confined to the lower

animals; but with some few birds the beak of the male differs from

that of the female. In the Huia of New Zealand the difference is

wonderfully great, and we hear from Dr. Buller*(3) that the male

uses his strong beak in chiselling the larvae of insects out of

decayed wood, whilst the female probes the softer parts with her far

longer, much curved and pliant beak: and thus they mutually aid each

other. In most cases, differences of structure between the sexes are

more or less directly connected with the propagation of the species:

thus a female, which has to nourish a multitude of ova, requires

more food than the male, and consequently requires special means for

procuring it. A male animal, which lives for a very short time,

might lose its organs for procuring food through disuse, without

detriment; but he would retain his locomotive organs in a perfect

state, so that he might reach the female. The female, on the other

hand, might safely lose her organs for flying, swimming, or walking,

if she gradually acquired habits which rendered such powers useless.



  * Westwood, Modern Classification of Insects, vol. ii., 1840, p.

541. For the statement about Tanais, mentioned below, I am indebted to

Fritz Muller.

  *(2) Kirby and Spence, Introduction to Entomology, vol. iii.,

1826, p. 309.

  *(3) Birds of New Zealand, 1872, p. 66.



  We are, however, here concerned only with sexual selection. This

depends on the advantage which certain individuals have over others of

the same sex and species solely in respect of reproduction. When, as

in the cases above mentioned, the two sexes differ in structure in

relation to different habits of life, they have no doubt been modified

through natural selection, and by inheritance, limited to one and

the same sex. So again the primary sexual organs, and those for

nourishing or protecting the young, come under the same influence; for

those individuals which generated or nourished their offspring best,

would leave, ceteris paribus, the greatest number to inherit their

superiority; whilst those which generated or nourished their offspring

badly, would leave but few to inherit their weaker powers. As the male

has to find the female, he requires organs of sense and locomotion,

but if these organs are necessary for the other purposes of life, as

is generally the case, they will have been developed through natural

selection. When the male has found the female, he sometimes absolutely

requires prehensile organs to hold her; thus Dr. Wallace informs me

that the males of certain moths cannot unite with the females if their

tarsi or feet are broken. The males of many oceanic crustaceans,

when adult, have their legs and antennae modified in an

extraordinary manner for the prehension of the female; hence we may

suspect that it is because these animals are washed about by the waves

of the open sea, that they require these organs in order to

propagate their kind, and if so, their development has been the result

of ordinary or natural selection. Some animals extremely low in the

scale have been modified for this same purpose; thus the males of

certain parasitic worms, when fully grown, have the lower surface of

the terminal part of their bodies roughened like a rasp, and with this

they coil round and permanently hold the females.*



  * M. Perrier advances this case (Revue Scientifique, Feb. 1, 1873,

p. 865) as one fatal to the belief in sexual election, inasmuch as

he supposes that I attribute all the differences between the sexes

to sexual selection. This distinguished naturalist, therefore, like so

many other Frenchmen, has not taken the trouble to understand even the

first principles of sexual selection. An English naturalist insists

that the claspers of certain male animals could not have been

developed through the choice of the female! Had I not met with this

remark, I should not have thought it possible for any one to have read

this chapter and to have imagined that I maintain that the choice of

the female had anything to do with the development of the prehensile

organs in the male.



  When the two sexes follow exactly the same habits of life, and the

male has the sensory or locomotive organs more highly developed than

those of the female, it may be that the perfection of these is

indispensable to the male for finding the female; but in the vast

majority of cases, they serve only to give one male an advantage

over another, for with sufficient time, the less well-endowed males

would succeed in pairing with the females; and judging from the

structure of the female, they would be in all other respects equally

well adapted for their ordinary habits of life. Since in such cases

the males have acquired their present structure, not from being better

fitted to survive in the struggle for existence, but from having

gained an advantage over other males, and from having transmitted this

advantage to their male offspring alone, sexual selection must here

have come into action. It was the importance of this distinction which

led me to designate this form of selection as Sexual Selection. So

again, if the chief service rendered to the male by his prehensile

organs is to prevent the escape of the female before the arrival of

other males, or when assaulted by them, these organs will have been

perfected through sexual selection, that is by the advantage

acquired by certain individuals over their rivals. But in most cases

of this kind it is impossible to distinguish between the effects of

natural and sexual selection. Whole chapters could be filled with

details on the differences between the sexes in their sensory,

locomotive, and prehensile organs. As, however, these structures are

not more interesting than others adapted for the ordinary purposes

of life, I shall pass them over almost entirely, giving only a few

instances under each class.

  There are many other structures and instincts which must have been

developed through sexual selection- such as the weapons of offence and

the means of defence- of the males for fighting with and driving

away their rivals- their courage and pugnacity- their various

ornaments- their contrivances for producing vocal or instrumental

music- and their glands for emitting odours, most of these latter

structures serving only to allure or excite the female. It is clear

that these characters are the result of sexual and not of ordinary

selection, since unarmed, unornamented, or unattractive males would

succeed equally well in the battle for life and in leaving a

numerous progeny, but for the presence of better endowed males. We may

infer that this would be the case, because the females, which are

unarmed and unornamented, are able to survive and procreate their

kind. Secondary sexual characters of the kind just referred to, will

be fully discussed in the following chapters, as being in many

respects interesting, but especially as depending on the will, choice,

and rivalry of the individuals of either sex. When we behold two males

fighting for the possession of the female, or several male birds

displaying their gorgeous plumage, and performing strange antics

before an assembled body of females, we cannot doubt that, though

led by instinct, they know what they are about, and consciously

exert their mental and bodily powers.

  Just as man can improve the breeds of his game-cocks by the

selection of those birds which are victorious in the cock-pit, so it

appears that the strongest and most vigorous males, or those

provided with the best weapons, have prevailed under nature, and

have led to the improvement of the natural breed or species. A

slight degree of variability leading to some advantage, however

slight, in reiterated deadly contests would suffice for the work of

sexual selection; and it is certain that secondary sexual characters

are eminently variable. Just as man can give beauty, according to

his standard of taste, to his male poultry, or more strictly can

modify the beauty originally acquired by the parent species, can

give to the Sebright bantam a new and elegant plumage, an erect and

peculiar carriage- so it appears that female birds in a state of

nature, have by a long selection of the more attractive males, added

to their beauty or other attractive qualities. No doubt this implies

powers of discrimination and taste on the part of the female which

will at first appear extremely improbable; but by the facts to be

adduced hereafter, I hope to be able to shew that the females actually

have these powers. When, however, it is said that the lower animals

have a sense of beauty, it must not be supposed that such sense is

comparable with that of a cultivated man, with his multiform and

complex associated ideas. A more just comparison would be between

the taste for the beautiful in animals, and that in the lowest

savages, who admire and deck themselves with any brilliant,

glittering, or curious object.

  From our ignorance on several points, the precise manner in which

sexual selections acts is somewhat uncertain. Nevertheless if those

naturalists who already believe in the mutability of species, will

read the following chapters, they will, I think, agree with me, that

sexual selection has played an important part in the history of the

organic world. It is certain that amongst almost all animals there

is a struggle between the males for the possession of the female. This

fact is so notorious that it would be superfluous to give instances.

Hence the females have the opportunity of selecting one out of several

males, on the supposition that their mental capacity suffices for

the exertion of a choice. In many cases special circumstances tend

to make the struggle between the males particularly severe. Thus the

males of our migratory birds generally arrive at their places of

breeding before the females, so that many males are ready to contend

for each female. I am informed by Mr. Jenner Weir, that the

bird-catchers assert that this is invariably the case with the

nightingale and blackcap, and with respect to the latter he can

himself confirm the statement.

  Mr. Swaysland of Brighton has been in the habit, during the last

forty years, of catching our migratory birds on their first arrival,

and he has never known the females of any species to arrive before

their males. During one spring he shot thirty-nine males of Ray's

wagtail (Budytes raii) before he saw a single female. Mr. Gould has

ascertained by the dissection of those snipes which arrive the first

in this country, that the males come before the females. And the

like holds good with most of the migratory birds of the United

States.* The majority of the male salmon in our rivers, on coming up

from the sea, are ready to breed before the females. So it appears

to be with frogs and toads. Throughout the great class of insects

the males almost always are the first to emerge from the pupal

state, so that they generally abound for a time before any females can

be seen.*(2) The cause of this difference between the males and

females in their periods of arrival and maturity is sufficiently

obvious. Those males which annually first migrated into any country,

or which in the spring were first ready to breed, or were the most

eager, would leave the largest number of offspring; and these would

tend to inherit similar instincts and constitutions. It must be

borne in mind that it would have been impossible to change very

materially the time of sexual maturity in the females, without at

the same time interfering with the period of the production of the

young- a period which must be determined by the seasons of the year.

On the whole there can be no doubt that with almost all animals, in

which the sexes are separate, there is a constantly recurrent struggle

between the males for the possession of the females.



  * J. A. Allen, on the "Mammals and Winter Birds of Florida,"

Bulletin of Comparative Zoology, Harvard College, p. 268.

  *(2) Even with those plants in which the sexes are separate, the

male flowers are generally mature before the female. As first shewn by

C. K. Sprengel, many hermaphrodite plants are dichogamous; that is,

their male and female organs are not ready at the same time, so that

they cannot be self-fertilised. Now in such flowers, the pollen is

in general matured before the stigma, though there are exceptional

cases in which the female organs are before-hand.



  Our difficulty in regard to sexual selection lies in understanding

how it is that the males which conquer other males, or those which

prove the most attractive to the females, leave a greater number of

offspring to inherit their superiority than their beaten and less

attractive rivals. Unless this result does follow, the characters

which give to certain males an advantage over others, could not be

perfected and augmented through sexual selection. When the sexes exist

in exactly equal numbers, the worst-endowed males will (except where

polygamy prevails), ultimately find females, and leave as many

offspring, as well fitted for their general habits of life, as the

best-endowed males. From various facts and considerations, I

formerly inferred that with most animals, in which secondary sexual

characters are well developed, the males considerably exceeded the

females in number; but this is not by any means always true. If the

males were to the females as two to one, or as three to two, or even

in a somewhat lower ratio, the whole affair would be simple; for the

better-armed or more attractive males would leave the largest number

of offspring. But after investigating, as far as possible, the

numerical proportion of the sexes, I do not believe that any great

inequality in number commonly exists. In most cases sexual selection

appears to have been effective in the following manner.

  Let us take any species, a bird for instance, and divide the females

inhabiting a district into two equal bodies, the one consisting of the

more vigorous and better-nourished individuals, and the other of the

less vigorous and healthy. The former, there can be little doubt,

would be ready to breed in the spring before the others; and this is

the opinion of Mr. Jenner Weir, who has carefully attended to the

habits of birds during many years. There can also be no doubt that the

most vigorous, best-nourished and earliest breeders would on an

average succeed in rearing the largest number of fine offspring.*

The males, as we have seen, are generally ready to breed before the

females; the strongest, and with some species the best armed of the

males, drive away the weaker; and the former would then unite with the

more vigorous and better-nourished females, because they are the first

to breed.*(2) Such vigorous pairs would surely rear a larger number of

offspring than the retarded females, which would be compelled to unite

with the conquered and less powerful males, supposing the sexes to

be numerically equal; and this is all that is wanted to add, in the

course of successive generations, to the size, strength and courage of

the males, or to improve their weapons.



  * Here is excellent evidence on the character of the offspring

from an experienced ornithologist. Mr. J. A. Allen, in speaking

(Mammals and Winter Birds of E. Florida, p. 229) of the later

broods, after the accidental destruction of the first, says, that

these "are found to be smaller and paler-coloured than those hatched

earlier in the season. In cases where several broods are reared each

year, as a general rule the birds of the earlier broods seem in all

respects the most perfect and vigorous."

  *(2) Hermann Muller has come to this same conclusion with respect to

those female bees which are the first to emerge from the pupa each

year. See his remarkable essay, "Anwendung der Darwin'schen Lehre

auf Bienen," Verh. d. V. Jahrg., xxix., p. 45



  But in very many cases the males which conquer their rivals, do

not obtain possession of the females, independently of the choice of

the latter. The courtship of animals is by no means so simple and

short an affair as might be thought. The females are most excited

by, or prefer pairing with, the more ornamented males, or those

which are the best songsters, or play the best antics; but it is

obviously probable that they would at the same time prefer the more

vigorous and lively males, and this has in some cases been confirmed

by actual observation.* Thus the more vigorous females, which are

the first to breed, will have the choice of many males; and though

they may not always select the strongest or best armed, they will

select those which are vigorous and well armed, and in other

respects the most attractive. Both sexes, therefore, of such early

pairs would as above explained, have an advantage over others in

rearing offspring; and this apparently has sufficed during a long

course of generations to add not only to the strength and fighting

powers of the males, but likewise to their various ornaments or

other attractions.



  * With respect to poultry, I have received information, hereafter to

be given, to this effect. Even birds, such as pigeons, which pair

for life, the female, as I hear from Mr. Jenner Weir, will desert

her mate if he is injured or grows weak.



  In the converse and much rarer case of the males selecting

particular females, it is plain that those which were the most

vigorous and had conquered others, would have the freest choice; and

it is almost certain that they would select vigorous as well as

attractive females. Such pairs would have an advantage in rearing

offspring, more especially if the male had the power to defend the

female during the pairing-season as occurs with some of the higher

animals, or aided her in providing for the young. The same

principles would apply if each sex preferred and selected certain

individuals of the opposite sex; supposing that they selected not only

the more attractive, but likewise the more vigorous individuals.



  Numerical Proportion of the Two Sexes.- I have remarked that

sexual selection would be a simple affair if the males were

considerably more numerous than the females. Hence I was led to

investigate, as far as I could, the proportions between the two

sexes of as many animals as possible; but the materials are scanty.

I will here give only a brief abstract of the results, retaining the

details for a supplementary discussion, so as not to interfere with

the course of my argument. Domesticated animals alone afford the means

of ascertaining the proportional numbers at birth; but no records have

been specially kept for this purpose. By indirect means, however, I

have collected a considerable body of statisties, from which it

appears that with most of our domestic animals the sexes are nearly

equal at birth. Thus 25,560 births of race-horses have been recorded

during twenty-one years, and the male births were to the female births

as 99.7 to 100. In greyhounds the inequality is greater than with

any other animal, for out of 6878 births during twelve years, the male

births were to the female as 110.1 to 100. It is, however, in some

degree doubtful whether it is safe to infer that the proportion

would be the same under natural conditions as under domestication; for

slight and unknown differences in the conditions affect the proportion

of the sexes. Thus with mankind, the male births in England are as

104.5, in Russia as 108.9, and with the Jews of Livonia as 120, to 100

female births. But I shall recur to this curious point of the excess

of male births in the supplement to this chapter. At the Cape of

Good Hope, however, male children of European extraction have been

born during several years in the proportion of between 90 and 99 to

100 female children.

  For our present purpose we are concerned with the proportions of the

sexes, not only at birth, but also at maturity, and this adds

another element of doubt; for it is a well-ascertained fact that

with man the number of males dying before or during birth, and

during the first two years of infancy, is considerably larger than

that of females. So it almost certainly is with male lambs, and

probably with some other animals. The males of some species kill one

another by fighting; or they drive one another about until they become

greatly emaciated. They must also be often exposed to various dangers,

whilst wandering about in eager search for the females. In many

kinds of fish the males are much smaller than the females, and they

are believed often to be devoured by the latter, or by other fishes.

The females of some birds appear to die earlier than the males; they

are also liable to be destroyed on their nests, or whilst in charge of

their young. With insects the female larvae are often larger than

those of the males, and would consequently be more likely to be

devoured. In some cases the mature females are less active and less

rapid in their movements than the males, and could not escape so

well from danger. Hence, with animals in a state of nature, we must

rely on mere estimation, in order to judge of the proportions of the

sexes at maturity; and this is but little trustworthy, except when the

inequality is strongly marked. Nevertheless, as far as a judgment

can be formed, we may conclude from the facts given in the supplement,

that the males of some few mammals, of many birds, of some fish and

insects, are considerably more numerous than the females.

  The proportion between the sexes fluctuates slightly during

successive years: thus with race-horses, for every 100 mares born

the stallions varied from 107.1 in one year to 92.6 in another year,

and with greyhounds from 116.3 to 95.3. But had larger numbers been

tabulated throughout an area more extensive than England, these

fluctuations would probably have disappeared; and such as they are,

would hardly suffice to lead to effective sexual selection in a

state of nature. Nevertheless, in the cases of some few wild

animals, as shewn in the supplement, the proportions seem to fluctuate

either during different seasons or in different localities in a

sufficient degree to lead to such selection. For it should be observed

that any advantage, gained during certain years or in certain

localities by those males which were able to conquer their rivals,

or were the most attractive to the females, would probably be

transmitted to the offspring, and would not subsequently be

eliminated. During the succeeding seasons, when, from the equality

of the sexes, every male was able to procure a female, the stronger or

more attractive males previously produced would still have at least as

good a chance of leaving offspring as the weaker or less attractive.



  Polygamy.- The practice of polygamy leads to the same results as

would follow from an actual inequality in the number of the sexes; for

if each male secures two or more females, many males cannot pair;

and the latter assuredly will be the weaker or less attractive

individuals. Many mammals and some few birds are polygamous, but

with animals belonging to the lower classes I have found no evidence

of this habit. The intellectual powers of such animals are, perhaps,

not sufficient to lead them to collect and guard a harem of females.

That some relation exists between polygamy and the development of

secondary sexual characters, appears nearly certain; and this supports

the view that a numerical preponderance of males would be eminently

favourable to the action of sexual selection. Nevertheless many

animals, which are strictly monogamous, especially birds, display

strongly-marked secondary sexual characters; whilst some few

animals, which are polygamous, do not have such characters.

  We will first briefly run through the mammals, and then turn to

birds. The gorilla seems to be polygamous, and the male differs

considerably from the female; so it is with some baboons, which live

in herds containing twice as many adult females as males. In South

America the Mycetes caraya present well-marked sexual differences,

in colour, beard, and vocal organs; and the male generally lives

with two or three wives: the male of the Cebus capucinus differs

somewhat from the female, and appears to be polygamous.* Little is

known on this head with respect to most other monkeys, but some

species are strictly monogamous. The ruminants are eminently

polygamous, and they present sexual differences more frequently than

almost any other group of mammals; this holds good, especially in

their weapons, but also in other characters. Most deer, cattle, and

sheep are polygamous; as are most antelopes, though some are

monogamous. Sir Andrew Smith, in speaking of the antelopes of South

Africa, says that in herds of about a dozen there was rarely more than

one mature male. The Asiatic Antilope saiga appears to be the most

inordinate polygamist in the world; for Pallas*(2) states that the

male drives away all rivals, and collects a herd of about a hundred

females and kids together; the female is hornless and has softer hair,

but does not otherwise differ much from the male. The wild horse of

the Falkland Islands and of the western states of N. America is

polygamous, but, except in his greater size and in the proportions

of his body, differs but little from the mare. The wild boar

presents well-marked sexual characters, in his great tusks and some

other points. In Europe and in India he leads a solitary life,

except during the breeding-season; but as is believed by Sir W.

Elliot, who has had many opportunities in India of observing this

animal, he consorts at this season with several females. Whether

this holds good in Europe is doubtful, but it is supported by some

evidence. The adult male Indian elephant, like the boar, passes much

of his time in solitude; but as Dr. Campbell states, when with others,

"It is rare to find more than one male with a whole herd of

females"; the larger males expelling or killing the smaller and weaker

ones. The male differs from the female in his immense tusks, greater

size, strength, and endurance; so great is the difference in these

respects that the males when caught are valued at one-fifth more

than the females.*(3) The sexes of other pachydermatous animals differ

very little or not at all, and, as far as known, they are not

polygamists. Nor have I heard of any species in the Orders of

Cheiroptera, Edentata, Insectivora and rodents being polygamous,

excepting that amongst the rodents, the common rat, according to

some rat-catchers, lives with several females. Nevertheless the two

sexes of some sloths (Edentata) differ in the character and colour

of certain patches of hair on their shoulders.*(4) And many kinds of

bats (Cheiroptera) present well-marked sexual differences, chiefly

in the males possessing odoriferous glands and pouches, and by their

being of a lighter colour.*(5) In the great order of rodents, as far

as I can learn, the sexes rarely differ, and when they do so, it is

but slightly in the tint of the fur.



  * On the Gorilla, Savage and Wyman, Boston Journal of Natural

History, vol. v., 1845-47, p. 423. On Cynocephalus, Brehm,

Illustriertes Thierleben, B. i., 1864, s. 77. On Mycetes, Rengger,

Naturgeschichte der Saugethiere von Paraguay, 1830, ss. 14, 20. On

Cebus, Brehm, ibid., s. 108.

  *(2) Pallas, Spicilegia Zoolog., fasc. xii., 1777, p. 29. Sir Andrew

Smith, Illustrations of the Zoology of S. Africa, 1849, pl. 29, on the

Kobus. Owen, in his Anatomy of Vertebrates (vol. iii., 1868, p. 633)

gives a table shewing incidentally which species of antelopes are

gregarious.

  *(3) Dr. Campbell, in Proc., Zoolog. Soc., 1869, p. 138. See also an

interesting paper by Lieut. Johnstone, in Proceedings, Asiatic Society

of Bengal, May, 1868.

  *(4) Dr. Gray, in Annals and Magazine of Natural History, 1871, p.

302.

  *(5) See Dr. Dobson's excellent paper in Proceedings of the

Zoological Society, 1873, p. 241.



  As I hear from Sir Andrew Smith, the lion in South Africa

sometimes lives with a single female, but generally with more, and, in

one case, was found with as many as five females; so that he is

polygamous. As far as I can discover, he is the only polygamist

amongst all the terrestrial Carnivora, and he alone presents

well-marked sexual characters. If, however, we turn to the marine

Carnivora, as we shall hereafter see, the case is widely different;

for many species of seals offer extraordinary sexual differences,

and they are eminently polygamous. Thus, according to Peron, the

male sea-elephant of the southern ocean always possesses several

females, and the sea-lion of Forster is said to be surrounded by

from twenty to thirty females. In the North, the male sea-bear of

Steller is accompanied by even a greater number of females. It is an

interesting fact, as Dr. Gill remarks,* that in the monogamous

species, "or those living in small communities, there is little

difference in size between the males and females; in the social

species, or rather those of which the males have harems, the males are

vastly larger than the females."



  * "The Eared Seals," American Naturalist, vol. iv., Jan. 1871.



  Amongst birds, many species, the sexes of which differ greatly

from each other, are certainly monogamous. In Great Britain we see

well-marked sexual differences, for instance, in the wild-duck which

pairs with a single female, the common blackbird, and the bullfinch

which is said to pair for life. I am informed by Mr. Wallace that

the like is true of chatterers or Cotingidae of South America, and

of many other birds. In several groups I have not been able to

discover whether the species are polygamous or monogamous. Lesson says

that birds of paradise, so remarkable for their sexual differences,

are polygamous, but Mr. Wallace doubts whether he had sufficient

evidence. Mr. Salvin tells me he has been led to believe that

humming-birds are polygamous. The male widow-bird, remarkable for

his caudal plumes, certainly seems to be a polygamist.* I have been

assured by Mr. Jenner Weir and by others, that it is somewhat common

for three starlings to frequent the same nest; but whether this is a

case of polygamy or polyandry has not been ascertained.



  * The Ibis, vol. iii., 1861, p. 133, on the Progne widow-bird. See

also on the Vidua axillaris, ibid., vol. ii., 1860, p. 211. On the

polygamy of the capercailzie and great bustard, see L. Lloyd, Game

Birds of Sweden, 1867, pp. 19, and 182. Montagu and Selby speak of the

black grouse as polygamous and of the red grouse as monogamous.



  The Gallinaceae exhibit almost as strongly marked sexual differences

as birds of paradise or humming-birds, and many of the species are, as

is well know, polygamous; others being strictly monogamous. What a

contrast is presented between the sexes of the polygamous peacock or

pheasant, and the monogamous guinea-fowl or partridge! Many similar

cases could be given, as in the grouse tribe, in which the males of

the polygamous capercailzie and black-cock differ greatly from the

females; whilst the sexes of the monogamous red grouse and ptarmigan

differ very little. In the Cursores, except amongst the bustards,

few species offer strongly-marked sexual differences, and the great

bustard (Otis tarda) is said to be polygamous. With the Grallatores,

extremely few species differ sexually, but the ruff (Machetes

pugnax) affords a marked exception, and this species is believed by

Montagu to be a polygamist. Hence it appears that amongst birds

there often exists a close relation between polygamy and the

development of strongly-marked sexual differences. I asked Mr.

Bartlett, of the Zoological Gardens, who has had very large experience

with birds, whether the male tragopan (one of the Gallinaceae) was

polygamous, and I was struck by his answering, "I do not know, but

should think so from his splendid colours."

  It deserves notice that the instinct of pairing with a single female

is easily lost under domestication. The wild-duck is strictly

monogamous, the domestic-duck highly polygamous. The Rev. W. D. Fox

informs me that out of some half-tamed wild-ducks, on a large pond

in his neighborhood, so many mallards were shot by the game-keeper

that only one was left for every seven or eight females; yet unusually

large broods were reared. The guinea-fowl is strictly monogamous;

but Mr. Fox finds that his birds succeed best when he keeps one cock

to two or three hens. Canary-birds pair in a state of nature, but

the breeders in England successfully put one male to four or five

females. I have noticed these cases, as rendering it probable that

wild monogamous species might readily become either temporarily or

permanently polygamous.

  Too little is known of the habits of reptiles and fishes to enable

us to speak of their marriage arrangements. The stickle-back

(Gasterosteus), however, is said to be a polygamist;* and the male

during the breeding-season differs conspicuously from the female.



  * Noel Humphreys, River Gardens, 1857.



  To sum up on the means through which, as far as we can judge, sexual

selection has led to the development of secondary sexual characters.

It has been shewn that the largest number of vigorous offspring will

be reared from the pairing of the strongest and best-armed males,

victorious in contests over other males, with the most vigorous and

best-nourished females, which are the first to breed in the spring. If

such females select the more attractive, and at the same time vigorous

males, they will rear a larger number of offspring than the retarded

females, which must pair with the less vigorous and less attractive

males. So it will be if the more vigorous males select the more

attractive and at the same time healthy and vigorous females; and this

will especially hold good if the male defends the female, and aids

in providing food for the young. The advantage thus gained by the more

vigorous pairs in rearing a larger number of offspring has

apparently sufficed to render sexual selection efficient. But a

large numerical preponderance of males over females will be still more

efficient; whether the preponderance is only occasional and local,

or permanent; whether it occurs at birth, or afterwards from the

greater destruction of the females; or whether it indirectly follows

from the practice of polygamy.



  The Male generally more modified than the Female.- Throughout the

animal kingdom, when the sexes differ in external appearance, it is,

with rare exceptions, the male which has been the more modified;

for, generally, the female retains a closer resemblance to the young

of her own species, and to other adult members of the same group.

The cause of this seems to lie in the males of almost all animals

having stronger passions than the females. Hence it is the males

that fight together and sedulously display their charms before the

females; and the victors transmit their superiority to their male

offspring. Why both sexes do not thus acquire the characters of

their fathers, will be considered hereafter. That the males of all

mammals eagerly pursue the females is notorious to every one. So it is

with birds; but many cock birds do not so much pursue the hen, as

display their plumage, perform strange antics, and pour forth their

song in her presence. The male in the few fish observed seems much

more eager than the female; and the same is true of alligators, and

apparently of batrachians. Throughout the enormous class of insects,

as Kirby remarks, "the law is that the male shall seek the female."*

Two good authorities, Mr. Blackwell and Mr. C. Spence Bate, tell me

that the males of spiders and crustaceans are more active and more

erratic in their habits than the females. When the organs of sense

or locomotion are present in the one sex of insects and crustaceans

and absent in the other, or when, as is frequently the case, they

are more highly developed in the one than in the other, it is, as

far as I can discover, almost invariably the male which retains such

organs, or has them most developed; and this shews that the male is

the more active member in the courtship of the sexes.*(2)



  * Kirby and Spence, Introduction to Entomology, vol. iii., 1826,

p. 342.

  *(2) One parasitic hymenopterous insect (Westwood, Modern Class.

of Insects, vol. ii., p. 160) forms an exception to the rule, as the

male has rudimentary wings, and never quits the cell in which it is

born, whilst the female has well-developed wings. Audouin believes

that the females of this species are impregnated by the males which

are born in the same cells with them; but it is much more probable

that the females visit other cells, so that close inter-breeding is

thus avoided. We shall hereafter meet in various classes, with a few

exceptional cases, in which the female, instead of the male, is the

seeker and wooer.



  The female, on the other hand, with the rarest exceptions, is less

eager than the male. As the illustrious Hunter* long ago observed, she

generally "requires to be courted"; she is coy, and may often be

seen endeavouring for a long time to escape from the male. Every

observer of the habits of animals will be able to call to mind

instances of this kind. It is shown by various facts, given hereafter,

and by the results fairly attributable to sexual selection, that the

female, though comparatively passive, generally exerts some choice and

accepts one male in preference to others. Or she may accept, as

appearances would sometimes lead us to believe, not the male which

is the most attractive to her, but the one which is the least

distasteful. The exertion of some choice on the part of the female

seems a law almost as general as the eagerness of the male.



  * Essays and Observations, edited by Owen, vol. i., 1861, p. 194.



  We are naturally led to enquire why the male, in so many and such

distinct classes, has become more eager than the female, so that he

searches for her, and plays the more active part in courtship. It

would be no advantage and some loss of power if each sex searched

for the other; but why should the male almost always be the seeker?

The ovules of plants after fertilisation have to be nourished for a

time; hence the pollen is necessarily brought to the female organs-

being placed on the stigma, by means of insects or the wind, or by the

spontaneous movements of the stamens; and in the Algae, &c., by the

locomotive power of the antherozooids. With lowly-organised aquatic

animals, permanently affixed to the same spot and having their sexes

separate, the male element is invariably brought to the female; and of

this we can see the reason, for even if the ova were detached before

fertilisation, and did not require subsequent nourishment or

protection, there would yet be greater difficulty in transporting them

than the male element, because, being larger than the latter, they are

produced in far smaller numbers. So that many of the lower animals

are, in this respect, analogous with plants.* The males of affixed and

aquatic animals having been led to emit their fertilising element in

this way, it is natural that any of their descendants, which rose in

the scale and became locomotive, should retain the same habit; and

they would approach the female as closely as possible, in order not to

risk the loss of the fertilising element in a long passage of it

through the water. With some few of the lower animals, the females

alone are fixed, and the males of these must be the seekers. But it is

difficult to understand why the males of species, of which the

progenitors were primordially free, should invariably have acquired

the habit of approaching the females, instead of being approached by

them. But in all cases, in order that the males should seek

efficiently, it would be necessary that they should be endowed with

strong passions; and the acquirement of such passions would

naturally follow from the more eager leaving a larger number of

offspring than the less eager.



  * Prof. Sachs (Lehrbuch der Botanik, 1870, S. 633) in speaking of

the male and female reproductive cells, remarks, "verhalt sich die

eine bei der Vereinigung activ,... die andere erscheint bei der

Vereinigung passiv."



  The great eagerness of the males has thus indirectly led to their

much more frequently developing secondary sexual characters than the

females. But the development of such characters would be much aided,

if the males were more liable to vary than the females- as I concluded

they were- after a long study of domesticated animals. Von

Nathusius, who has had very wide experience, is strongly of the same

opinion.* Good evidence also in favour of this conclusion can be

produced by a comparison of the two sexes in mankind. During the

Novara expedition*(2) a vast number of measurements was made of

various parts of the body in different races, and the men were found

in almost every case to present a greater range of variation than

the women; but I shall have to recur to this subject in a future

chapter. Mr. J. Wood,*(3) who has carefully attended to the

variation of the muscles in man, puts in italics the conclusion that

"the greatest number of abnormalities in each subject is found in

the males." He had previously remarked that "altogether in 102

subjects, the varieties of redundancy were found to be half as many

again as in females, contrasting widely with the greater frequency

of deficiency in females before described." Professor Macalister

likewise remarks*(4) that variations in the muscles "are probably more

common in males than females." Certain muscles which are not

normally present in mankind are also more frequently developed in

the male than in the female sex, although exceptions to this rule

are said to occur. Dr. Burt Wilder*(5) has tabulated the cases of

152 individuals with supernumerary digits, of which 86 were males, and

39, or less than half, females, the remaining 27 being of unknown sex.

It should not, however, be overlooked that women would more frequently

endeavour to conceal a deformity of this kind than men. Again, Dr.

L. Meyer asserts that the ears of man are more variable in form than

those of a woman.*(6) Lastly the temperature is more variable in man

than in woman.*(7)



  * Vortrage uber Viehzucht, 1872, p. 63.

  *(2) Reise der Novara: Anthropolog. Theil, 1867, ss. 216-269. The

results were calculated by Dr. Weisbach from measurements made by Drs.

K. Scherzer and Schwarz. On the greater variability of the males of

domesticated animals, see my Variation of Animals and Plants under

Domestication, vol. ii., 1868, p. 75.

  *(3) Proceedings of the Royal Society, vol. xvi., July, 1868, pp.

519 and 524.

  *(4) Proc. Royal Irish Academy, vol. x., 1868, p. 123.

  *(5) Mass. Medical Society, ii., No. 3, 1868, p. 9.

  *(6) Archiv fur Path. Anat. und Phys., 1871, p. 488.

  *(7) The conclusions recently arrived at by Dr. J. Stockton Hough,

on the temperature of man, are given in the Pop. Sci. Review, Jan.

1, 1874, p. 97.



 The cause of the greater general variability in the male sex, than in

the female is unknown, except in so far as secondary sexual characters

are extraordinarily variable, and are usually confined to the males;

and, as we shall presently see, this fact is, to a certain extent,

intelligible. Through the action of sexual and natural selection

male animals have been rendered in very many instances widely

different from their females; but independently of selection the two

sexes, from differing constitutionally, tend to vary in a somewhat

different manner. The female has to expend much organic matter in

the formation of her ova, whereas the male expends much force in

fierce contests with his rivals, in wandering about in search of the

female, in exerting his voice, pouring out odoriferous secretions,

&c.: and this expenditure is generally concentrated within a short

period. The great vigour of the male during the season of love seems

often to intensify his colours, independently of any marked difference

from the female.* In mankind, and even as low down in the organic

scale as in the Lepidoptera, the temperature of the body is higher

in the male than in the female, accompanied in the case of man by a

slower pulse.*(2) On the whole the expenditure of matter and force

by the two sexes is probably nearly equal, though effected in very

different ways and at different rates.



  * Prof. Mantegazza is inclined to believe ("Lettera a Carlo Darwin,"

Archivio per l'Anthropologia, 1871, p. 306) that the bright colours,

common in so many male animals, are due to the presence and

retention by them of the spermatic fluid; but this can hardly be the

case; for many male birds, for instance young pheasants, become

brightly coloured in the autumn of their first year.

  *(2) For mankind, see Dr. J. Stockton Hough, whose conclusions are

given in the Popular Science Review, 1874, p. 97. See Girard's

observations on the Lepidoptera, as given in the Zoological Record,

1869, p. 347.



  From the causes just specified the two sexes can hardly fail to

differ somewhat in constitution, at least during the

breeding-season; and, although they may be subjected to exactly the

same conditions, they will tend to vary in a different manner. If such

variations are of no service to either sex, they will not be

accumulated and increased by sexual or natural selection.

Nevertheless, they may become permanent if the exciting cause acts

permanently; and in accordance with a frequent form of inheritance

they may be transmitted to that sex alone in which they first

appeared. In this case the two sexes will come to present permanent,

yet unimportant, differences of character. For instance, Mr. Allen

shews that with a large number of birds inhabiting the northern and

southern United States, the specimens from the south are

darker-coloured than those from the north; and this seems to be the

direct result of the difference in temperature, light, &c., between

the two regions. Now, in some few cases, the two sexes of the same

species appear to have been differently affected; in the Agelaeus

phoeniceus the males have had their colours greatly intensified in the

south; whereas with Cardinalis virginianus it is the females which

have been thus affected; with Quiscalus major the females have been

rendered extremely variable in tint, whilst the males remain nearly

uniform.*



  * Mammals and Birds of E. Florida, pp. 234, 280, 295.



  A few exceptional cases occur in various classes of animals, in

which the females instead of the males have acquired well pronounced

secondary sexual characters, such as brighter colours, greater size,

strength, or pugnacity. With birds there has sometimes been a complete

transposition of the ordinary characters proper to each sex; the

females having become the more eager in courtship, the males remaining

comparatively passive, but apparently selecting the more attractive

females, as we may infer from the results. Certain hen birds have thus

been rendered more brightly coloured or otherwise ornamented, as

well as more powerful and pugnacious than the cocks; these

characters being transmitted to the female offspring alone.

  It may be suggested that in some cases a double process of selection

has been carried on; that the males have selected the more

attractive females, and the latter the more attractive males. This

process, however, though it might lead to the modification of both

sexes, would not make the one sex different from the other, unless

indeed their tastes for the beautiful differed; but this is a

supposition too improbable to be worth considering in the case of

any animal, excepting man. There are, however, many animals in which

the sexes resemble each other, both being furnished with the same

ornaments, which analogy would lead us to attribute to the agency of

sexual selection. In such cases it may be suggested with more

plausibility, that there has been a double or mutual process of sexual

selection; the more vigorous and precocious females selecting the more

attractive and vigorous males, the latter rejecting all except the

more attractive females. But from what we know of the habits of

animals, this view is hardly probable, for the male is generally eager

to pair with any female. It is more probable that the ornaments common

to both sexes were acquired by one sex, generally the male, and then

transmitted to the offspring of both sexes. If, indeed, during a

lengthened period the males of any species were greatly to exceed

the females in number, and then during another lengthened period,

but under different conditions, the reverse were to occur, a double,

but not simultaneous, process of sexual selection might easily be

carried on, by which the two sexes might be rendered widely different.

  We shall hereafter see that many animals exist, of which neither sex

is brilliantly coloured or provided with special ornaments, and yet

the members of both sexes or of one alone have probably acquired

simple colours, such as white or black, through sexual selection.

The absence of bright tints or other ornaments may be the result of

variations of the right kind never having occurred, or of the

animals themselves having preferred plain black or white. Obscure

tints have often been developed through natural selection for the sake

of protection, and the acquirement through sexual selection of

conspicuous colours, appears to have been sometimes checked from the

danger thus incurred. But in other cases the males during long ages

may have struggled together for the possession of the females, and yet

no effect will have been produced, unless a larger number of offspring

were left by the more successful males to inherit their superiority,

than by the less successful: and this, as previously shewn, depends on

many complex contingencies.

  Sexual selection acts in a less rigorous manner than natural

selection. The latter produces its effects by the life or death at all

ages of the more or less successful individuals. Death, indeed, not

rarely ensues from the conflicts of rival males. But generally the

less successful male merely fails to obtain a female, or obtains a

retarded and less vigorous female later in the season, or, if

polygamous, obtains fewer females; so that they leave fewer, less

vigorous, or no offspring. In regard to structures acquired through

ordinary or natural selection, there is in most cases, as long as

the conditions of life remain the same, a limit to the amount of

advantageous modification in relation to certain special purposes; but

in regard to structures adapted to make one male victorious over

another, either in fighting or in charming the female, there is no

definite limit to the amount of advantageous modification; so that

as long as the proper variations arise the work of sexual selection

will go on. This circumstance may partly account for the frequent

and extraordinary amount of variability presented by secondary

sexual characters. Nevertheless, natural selection will determine that

such characters shall not be acquired by the victorious males, if they

would be highly injurious, either by expending too much of their vital

powers, or by exposing them to any great danger. The development,

however, of certain structures- of the horns, for instance, in certain

stags- has been carried to a wonderful extreme; and in some cases to

an extreme which, as far as the general conditions of life are

concerned, must be slightly injurious to the male. From this fact we

learn that the advantages which favoured males derive from

conquering other males in battle or courtship, and thus leaving a

numerous progeny, are in the long run greater than those derived

from rather more perfect adaptation to their conditions of life. We

shall further see, and it could never have been anticipated, that

the power to charm the female has sometimes been more important than

the power to conquer other males in battle.



                    LAWS OF INHERITANCE.



  In order to understand how sexual selection has acted on many

animals of many classes, and in the course of ages has produced a

conspicuous result, it is necessary to bear in mind the laws of

inheritance, as far as they are known. Two distinct elements are

included under the term "inheritance"- the transmission, and the

development of characters; but as these generally go together, the

distinction is often overlooked. We see this distinction in those

characters which are transmitted through the early years of life,

but are developed only at maturity or during old age. We see the

same distinction more clearly with secondary sexual characters, for

these are transmitted through both sexes, though developed in one

alone. That they are present in both sexes, is manifest when two

species, having strongly-marked sexual characters, are crossed, for

each transmits the characters proper to its own male and female sex to

the hybrid offspring of either sex. The same fact is likewise

manifest, when characters proper to the male are occasionally

developed in the female when she grows old or becomes diseased, as,

for instance, when the common hen assumes the flowing tail-feathers,

hackles, comb, spurs, voice, and even pugnacity of the cock.

Conversely, the same thing is evident, more or less plainly, with

castrated males. Again, independently of old age or disease,

characters are occasionally transferred from the male to the female,

as when, in certain breeds of the fowl, spurs regularly appear in

the young and healthy females. But in truth they are simply

developed in the female; for in every breed each detail in the

structure of the spur is transmitted through the female to her male

offspring. Many cases will hereafter be given, where the female

exhibits, more or less perfectly, characters proper to the male, in

whom they must have been first developed, and then transferred to

the female. The converse case of the first development of characters

in the female and of transference to the male, is less frequent; it

will therefore be well to give one striking instance. With bees the

pollen-collecting apparatus is used by the female alone for

gathering pollen for the larvae, yet in most of the species it is

partially developed in the males to whom it is quite useless, and it

is perfectly developed in the males of Bombus or the humble-bee.* As

not a single other hymenopterous insect, not even the wasp, which is

closely allied to the bee, is provided with a pollen-collecting

apparatus, we have no grounds for supposing that male bees

primordially collected pollen as well as the females; although we have

some reason to suspect that male mammals primordially suckled their

young as well as the females. Lastly, in all cases of reversion,

characters are transmitted through two, three, or many more

generations, and are then developed under certain unknown favourable

conditions. This important distinction between transmission and

development will be best kept in mind by the aid of the hypothesis

of pangenesis. According to this hypothesis, every unit or cell of the

body throws off gemmules or undeveloped atoms, which are transmitted

to the offspring of both sexes, and are multiplied by self-division.

They may remain undeveloped during the early years of life or during

successive generations; and their development into units or cells,

like those from which they were derived, depends on their affinity

for, and union with other units or cells previously developed in the

due order of growth.



  * H. Muller, "Anwendung der Darwin'schen Lehre, &c.," Verh. d. n. V.

Jahrg., xxix. p. 42.



  Inheritance at corresponding Periods of Life.- This tendency is well

established. A new character, appearing in a young animal, whether

it lasts throughout life or is only transient, will, in general,

reappear in the offspring at the same age and last for the same

time. If, on the other hand, a new character appears at maturity, or

even during old age, it tends to reappear in the offspring at the same

advanced age. When deviations from this rule occur, the transmitted

characters much oftener appear before, than after the corresponding

age. As I have dwelt on this subject sufficiently in another work,*

I will here merely give two or three instances, for the sake of

recalling the subject to the reader's mind. In several breeds of the

fowl, the down-covered chickens, the young birds in their first true

plumage, and the adults differ greatly from one another, as well as

from their common parent-form, the Gallus bankiva; and these

characters are faithfully transmitted by each breed to their offspring

at the corresponding periods of life. For instance, the chickens of

spangled Hamburgs, whilst covered with down have a few dark spots on

the head and rump, but are not striped longitudinally, as in many

other breeds; in their first true plumage, "they are beautifully

pencilled," that is each feather is transversely marked by numerous

dark bars; but in their second plumage the feathers all become

spangled or tipped with a dark round spot.*(2) Hence in this breed

variations have occurred at, and  been transmitted to, three

distinct periods of life. The pigeon offers a more remarkable case,

because the aboriginal parent species does not undergo any change of

plumage with advancing age, excepting that at maturity the breast

becomes more iridescent; yet there are breeds which do not acquire

their characteristic colours until they have moulted two, three, or

four times; and these modifications of plumage are regularly

transmitted.



  * The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol. ii.,

1868, p. 75. In the last chapter but one, the provisional hypothesis

of pangenesis, above alluded to, is fully explained.

  *(2) These facts are given on the high authority of a great breeder,

Mr. Teebay; see Tegetmeier's Poultry Book, 1868, p. 158. On the

characters of chickens of different breeds, and on the breeds of the

pigeon, alluded to in the following paragraph, see Variation of

Animals, &c., vol. i., pp. 160, 249; vol. ii., p. 77.



  Inheritance at corresponding Seasons of the Year.- With animals in a

state of nature, innumerable instances occur of characters appearing

periodically at different seasons. We see this in the horns of the

stag, and in the fur of arctic animals which becomes thick and white

during the winter. Many birds acquire bright colours and other

decorations during the breeding-season alone. Pallas states,* that

in Siberia domestic cattle and horses become lighter-coloured during

the winter; and I have myself observed, and heard of similar

strongly marked changes of colour, that is, from brownish cream-colour

or reddish-brown to a perfect white, in several ponies in England.

Although I do not know that this tendency to change the colour of

the coat during different seasons is transmitted, yet it probably is

so, as all shades of colour are strongly inherited by the horse. Nor

is this form of inheritance, as limited by the seasons, more

remarkable than its limitation by age or sex.



  * Novae species Quadrupedum e Glirium ordine, 1778, p. 7. On the

transmission of colour by the horse, see Variation of Animals and

Plants under Domestication, vol. i., p. 51. Also vol. ii., p. 71,

for a general discussion on "Inheritance as limited by Sex."



  Inheritance as Limited by Sex.- The equal transmission of characters

to both sexes is the commonest form of inheritance, at least with

those animals which do not present strongly-marked sexual differences,

and indeed with many of these. But characters are somewhat commonly

transferred exclusively to that sex, in which they first appear. Ample

evidence on this head has been advanced in my work on Variation

under Domestication, but a few instances may here be given. There

are breeds of the sheep and goat, in which the horns of the male

differ greatly in shape from those of the female; and these

differences, acquired under domestication, are regularly transmitted

to the same sex. As a rule, it is the females alone in cats which

are tortoise-shell, the corresponding colour in the males being

rusty-red. With most breeds of the fowl, the characters proper to each

sex are transmitted to the same sex alone. So general is this form

of transmission that it is an anomaly when variations in certain

breeds are transmitted equally to both sexes. There are also certain

sub-breeds of the fowl in which the males can hardly be

distinguished from one another, whilst the females differ considerably

in colour. The sexes of the pigeon in the parent-species do not differ

in any external character; nevertheless, in certain domesticated

breeds the male is coloured differently from the female.* The wattle

in the English carrier pigeon, and the crop in the pouter, are more

highly developed in the male than in the female; and although these

characters have been gained through long-continued selection by man,

the slight differences between the sexes are wholly due to the form of

inheritance which has prevailed; for they have arisen, not from, but

rather in opposition to, the wish of the breeder.



  * Dr. Chapuis, Le Pigeon Voyageur Belge, 1865, p. 87. Boitard et

Corbie, Les Pigeons de Voliere, &c., 1824, p. 173. See, also, on

similar differences in certain breeds at Modena, Le variazioni dei

Colombi domestici, del Paolo Bonizzi, 1873.



  Most of our domestic races have been formed by the accumulation of

many slight variations; and as some of the successive steps have

been transmitted to one sex alone, and some to both sexes, we find

in the different breeds of the same species all gradations between

great sexual dissimilarity and complete similarity. Instances have

already been given with the breeds of the fowl and pigeon, and under

nature analogous cases are common. With animals under domestication,

but whether in nature I will not venture to say, one sex may lose

characters proper to it, and may thus come somewhat to resemble the

opposite sex; for instance, the males of some breeds of the fowl

have lost their masculine tail-plumes and hackles. On the other

hand, the differences between the sexes may be increased under

domestication, as with merino sheep, in which the ewes have lost their

horns. Again, characters proper to one sex may suddenly appear in

the other sex; as in those sub-breeds of the fowl in which the hens

acquire spurs whilst young; or, as in certain Polish sub-breeds, in

which the females, as there is reason to believe, originally

acquired a crest, and subsequently transferred it to the males. All

these cases are intelligible on the hypothesis of pangenesis; for they

depend on the gemmules of certain parts, although present in both

sexes, becoming, through the influence of domestication, either

dormant or developed in either sex.

  There is one difficult question which it will be convenient to defer

to a future chapter; namely, whether a character at first developed in

both sexes, could through selection be limited in its development to

one sex alone. If, for instance, a breeder observed that some of his

pigeons (of which the characters are usually transferred in an equal

degree to both sexes) varied into pale blue, could he by

long-continued selection make a breed, in which the males alone should

be of this tint, whilst the females remained unchanged? I will here

only say, that this, though perhaps not impossible, would be extremely

difficult; for the natural result of breeding from the pale-blue males

would be to change the whole stock of both sexes to this tint. If,

however, variations of the desired tint appeared, which were from

the first limited in their development to the male sex, there would

not be the least difficulty in making a breed with the two sexes of

a different colour, as indeed has been effected with a Belgian

breed, in which the males alone are streaked with black. In a

similar manner, if any variation appeared in a female pigeon, which

was from the first sexually limited in its development to the females,

it would be easy to make a breed with the females alone thus

characterised; but if the variation was not thus originally limited,

the process would be extremely difficult, perhaps impossible.*



  * Since the publication of the first edition of this work, it has

been highly satisfactory to me to find the following remarks (the

Field, Sept., 1872) from so experienced a breeder as Mr. Tegetmeier.

After describing some curious cases in pigeons, of the transmission of

colour by one sex alone, and the formation of a sub-breed with this

character, he says: "It is a singular circumstance that Mr. Darwin

should have suggested the possibility of modifying the sexual

colours of birds by a course of artificial selection. When he did

so, he was in ignorance of these facts that I have related; but it

is remarkable how very closely he suggested the right method of

procedure."



  On the Relation between the Period of Development of a Character and

its Transmission to one Sex or to both Sexes.- Why certain

characters should be inherited by both sexes, and other characters

by one sex alone, namely by that sex in which the character first

appeared, is in most cases quite unknown. We cannot even conjecture

why with certain sub-breeds of the pigeon, black striae, though

transmitted through the female, should be developed in the male alone,

whilst every other character is equally transferred to both sexes.

Why, again, with cats, the tortoise-shell colour should, with rare

exceptions, be developed in the female alone. The very same character,

such as deficient or super-numerary digits, colour-blindness, &c., may

with mankind be inherited by the males alone of one family, and in

another family by the females alone, though in both cases

transmitted through the opposite as well as through the same sex.*

Although we are thus ignorant, the two following rules seem often to

hold good- that variations which first appear in either sex at a

late period of life tend to be developed in the same sex alone; whilst

variations which first appear early in life in either sex tend to be

developed in both sexes. I am, however, far from supposing that this

is the sole determining cause. As I have not elsewhere discussed

this subject, and it has an important bearing on sexual selection, I

must here enter into lengthy and somewhat intricate details.



  * References are given in my Variation of Animals and Plants under

Domestication, vol. ii., p. 72.



  It is in itself probable that any character appearing at an early

age would tend to be inherited equally by both sexes, for the sexes do

not differ much in constitution before the power of reproduction is

gained. On the other hand, after this power has been gained and the

sexes have come to differ in constitution, the gemmules (if I may

again use the language of pangenesis) which are cast off from each

varying part in the one sex would be much more likely to possess the

proper affinities for uniting with the tissues of the same sex, and

thus becoming developed, than with those of the opposite sex.

  I was first led to infer that a relation of this kind exists, from

the fact that whenever and in whatever manner the adult male differs

from the adult female, he differs in the same manner from the young of

both sexes. The generality of this fact is quite remarkable: it

holds good with almost all mammals, birds, amphibians, and fishes;

also with many crustaceans, spiders, and some few insects, such as

certain Orthoptera and Libellulae. In all these cases the

variations, through the accumulation of which the male acquired his

proper masculine characters, must have occurred at a somewhat late

period of life; otherwise the young males would have been similarly

characterised; and conformably with our rule, the variations are

transmitted to and developed in the adult males alone. When, on the

other hand, the adult male closely resembles the young of both sexes

(these, with rare exceptions, being alike), he generally resembles the

adult female; and in most of these cases the variations through

which the young and old acquired their present characters, probably

occurred, according to our rule, during youth. But there is here

room for doubt, for characters are sometimes transferred to the

offspring at an earlier age than that at which they first appeared

in the parents, so that the parents may have varied when adult, and

have transferred their characters to their offspring whilst young.

There are, moreover, many animals, in which the two sexes closely

resemble each other, and yet both differ from their young: and here

the characters of the adults must have been acquired late in life;

nevertheless, these characters, in apparent contradiction to our rule,

are transferred to both sexes. We must not however, overlook the

possibility or even probability of successive variations of the same

nature occurring, under exposure to similar conditions, simultaneously

in both sexes at a rather late period of life; and in this case the

variations would be transferred to the offspring of both sexes at a

corresponding late age; and there would then be no real

contradiction to the rule that variations occurring late in life are

transferred exclusively to the sex in which they first appeared.

This latter rule seems to hold true more generally than the second

one, namely, that variations which occur in either sex early in life

tend to be transferred to both sexes. As it was obviously impossible

even to estimate in how large a number of cases throughout the

animal kingdom these two propositions held good, it occurred to me

to investigate some striking or crucial instances, and to rely on

the result.

  An excellent case for investigation is afforded by the deer

family. In all the species, but one, the horns are developed only in

the males, though certainly transmitted through the females, and

capable of abnormal development in them. In the reindeer, on the other

hand, the female is provided with horns; so that in this species,

the horns ought, according to our rule, to appear early in life,

long before the two sexes are mature and have come to differ much in

constitution. In all the other species the horns ought to appear later

in life, which would lead to their development in that sex alone, in

which they first appeared in the progenitor of the whole family. Now

in seven species, belonging to distinct sections of the family and

inhabiting different regions, in which the stags alone bear horns, I

find that the horns first appear at periods, varying from nine

months after birth in the roebuck to ten, twelve or even more months

in the stags of the six other and larger species.* But with the

reindeer the case is widely different; for, as I hear from Prof.

Nilsson, who kindly made enquiries for me in Lapland, the horns appear

in the young animals within four or five weeks after birth, and at the

same time in both sexes. So that here we have a structure, developed

at a most unusually early age in one species of the family, and

likewise common to both sexes in this one species alone.



  * I am much obliged to Mr. Cupples for having made enquiries for

me in regard to the roebuck and red deer of Scotland from Mr.

Robertson, the experienced head-forester to the Marquis of

Breadalbane. In regard to fallow-deer, I have to thank Mr. Eyton and

others for information. For the Cervus alces of N. America, see Land

and Water, 1868, pp. 221 and 254; and for the C. Virginianus and C.

strongyloceros of the same continent, see J. D. Caton, in Ottawa Acad.

of Nat. Sc., 1868, p. 13. For Cervus eldi of Pegu, see Lieut.

Beaven, Proccedings of the Zoological Society, 1867, p. 762.



  In several kinds of antelopes, only the males are provided with

horns, whilst in the greater number both sexes bear horns. With

respect to the period of development, Mr. Blyth informs me that

there was at one time in the Zoological Gardens a young koodoo (A.

strepsiceros), of which the males alone are horned, and also the young

of a closely-allied species, the eland (A. oreas), in which both sexes

are horned. Now it is in strict conformity with our rule, that in

the young male koodoo, although ten months old, the horns were

remarkably small, considering the size ultimately attained by them;

whilst in the young male eland, although only three months old, the

horns were already very much larger than in the koodoo. It is also a

noticeable fact that in the prong-horned antelope,* only a few of

the females, about one in five, have horns, and these are in a

rudimentary state, though sometimes above four inches long: so that as

far as concerns the possession of horns by the males alone, this

species is in an intermediate condition, and the horns do not appear

until about five or six months after birth. Therefore in comparison

with what little we know of the development of the horns in other

antelopes, and from what we do know with respect to the horns of deer,

cattle, &c., those of the prong-horned antelope appear at an

intermediate period of life,- that is, not very early, as in cattle

and sheep, nor very late, as in the larger deer and antelopes. The

horns of sheep, goats, and cattle, which are well developed in both

sexes, though not quite equal in size, can be felt, or even seen, at

birth or soon afterwards.*(2) Our rule, however, seems to fail in some

breeds of sheep, for instance merinos, in which the rams alone are

horned; for I cannot find on enquiry,*(3) that the horns are developed

later in life in this breed than in ordinary sheep in which both sexes

are horned. But with domesticated sheep the presence or absence of

horns is not a firmly fixed character; for a certain proportion of the

merino ewes bear small horns, and some of the rams are hornless; and

in most breeds hornless ewes are occasionally produced.



  * Antilocapra Americana. I have to thank Dr. Canfield for

information with respect to the horns of the female: see also his

paper in Proceedings of the Zoological Society, 1866, p. 109. Also

Owen, Anatomy of Vertebrates, vol. iii., p. 627.

  *(2) I have been assured that the horns of the sheep in north

Wales can always be felt, and are sometimes even an inch in length, at

birth. Youatt says (Cattle, 1834, p. 277), that the prominence of

the frontal bone in cattle penetrates the cutis at birth, and that the

horny matter is soon formed over it.

  *(3) I am greatly indebted to Prof. Victor Carus for having made

enquiries for me, from the highest authorities, with respect to the

merino sheep of Saxony. On the Guinea coast of Africa there is,

however, a breed of sheep in which, as with merinos, the rams alone

bear horns; and Mr. Winwood Reade informs me that in one case observed

by him, a young ram, born on Feb. 10th, first shewed horns on March

6th, so that in this instance, in conformity with rule, the

development of the horns occurred at a later period of life than in

Welsh sheep, in which both sexes are horned.



  Dr. W. Marshall has lately made a special study of the protuberances

so common on the heads of birds,* and he comes to the following

conclusion:- that with those species in which they are confined to the

males, they are developed late in life; whereas with those species

in which they are common to the two sexes, they are developed at a

very early period. This is certainly a striking confirmation of my two

laws of inheritance.



  * "Uber die knochernen Schadelhocker der Vogel", in the

Niederland. Archiv fur Zoologie, B.i., Heft 2, 1872.



  In most of the species of the splendid family of the pheasants,

the males differ conspicuously from the females, and they acquire

their ornaments at a rather late period of life. The eared pheasant

(Crossoptilon auritum), however, offers a remarkable exception, for

both sexes possess the fine caudal plumes, the large ear-tufts and the

crimson velvet about the head; I find that all these characters appear

very early in life in accordance with rule. The adult male can,

however, be distinguished from the adult female by the presence of

spurs; and conformably with our rule, these do not begin to be

developed before the age of six months, as I am assured by Mr.

Bartlett, and even at this age, the two sexes can hardly be

distinguished.* The male and female peacock differ conspicuously

from each other in almost every part of their plumage, except in the

elegant head-crest, which is common to both sexes; and this is

developed very early in life, long before the other ornaments, which

are confined to the male. The wild-duck offers an analogous case,

for the beautiful green speculum on the wings is common to both sexes,

though duller and somewhat smaller in the female, and it is

developed early in life, whilst the curled tail-feathers and other

ornaments of the male are developed later.*(2) Between such extreme

cases of close sexual resemblance and wide dissimilarity, as those

of the Crossoptilon and peacock, many intermediate ones could be

given, in which the characters follow our two rules in their order

of development.



  * In the common peacock (Pavo cristatus) the male alone possesses

spurs, whilst both sexes of the Java peacock (P. muticus) offer the

unusual case of being furnished with spurs. Hence I fully expected

that in the latter species they would have been developed earlier in

life than in the common peacock; but M. Hegt of Amsterdam informs

me, that with young birds of the previous year, of both species,

compared on April 23rd, 1869, there was no difference in the

development of the spurs. The spurs, however, were as yet

represented merely by slight knobs or elevations. I presume that I

should have been informed if any difference in the rate of development

had been observed subsequently.

  *(2) In some other species of the duck family the speculum differs

in a greater degree in the two sexes; but I have not been able to

discover whether its full development occurs later in life in the

males of such species, than in the male of the common duck, as ought

to be the case according to our rule. With the allied Mergus

cucullatus we have, however, a case of this kind: the two sexes differ

conspicuously in general plumage, and to a considerable degree in

the speculum, which is pure white in the male and greyish-white in the

female. Now the young males at first entirely resemble the females,

and have a greyish-white speculum, which becomes pure white at an

earlier age than that at which the adult male acquires his other and

more strongly-marked sexual differences: see Audubon, Ornithological

Biography, vol. iii., 1835, pp. 249-250.



  As most insects emerge from the pupal state in a mature condition,

it is doubtful whether the period of development can determine the

transference of their characters to one or to both sexes. But we do

not know that the coloured scales, for instance, in two species of

butterflies, in one of which the sexes differ in colour, whilst in the

other they are alike, are developed at the same relative age in the

cocoon. Nor do we know whether all the scales are simultaneously

developed on the wings of the same species of butterfly, in which

certain coloured marks are confined to one sex, whilst others are

common to both sexes. A difference of this kind in the period of

development is not so improbable as it may at first appear; for with

the Orthoptera, which assume their adult state, not by a single

metamorphosis, but by a succession of moults, the young males of

some species at first resemble the females, and acquire their

distinctive masculine characters only at a later moult. Strictly

analogous cases occur at the successive moults of certain male

crustaceans.

  We have as yet considered the transference of characters, relatively

to their period of development, only in species in a natural state; we

will now turn to domesticated animals, and first touch on

monstrosities and diseases. The presence of supernumerary digits,

and the absence of certain phalanges, must be determined at an early

embryonic period- the tendency to profuse bleeding is at least

congenital, as is probably colour-blindness- yet these

peculiarities, and other similar ones, are often limited in their

transmission to one sex; so that the rule that characters, developed

at an early period, tend to be transmitted to both sexes, here

wholly fails. But this rule, as before remarked, does not appear to be

nearly so general as the converse one, namely, that characters which

appear late in life in one sex are transmitted exclusively to the same

sex. From the fact of the above abnormal peculiarities becoming

attached to one sex, long before the sexual functions are active, we

may infer that there must be some difference between the sexes at an

extremely early age. With respect to sexually-limited diseases, we

know too little of the period at which they originate, to draw any

safe conclusion. Gout, however, seems to fall under our rule, for it

is generally caused by intemperance during manhood, and is transmitted

from the father to his sons in a much more marked manner than to his

daughters.

  In the various domestic breeds of sheeps, goats, and cattle, the

males differ from their respective females in the shape or development

of their horns, forehead, mane, dewlap, tail, and hump on the

shoulders; and these peculiarities, in accordance with our rule, are

not fully developed until a rather late period of life. The sexes of

dogs do not differ, except that in certain breeds, especially in the

Scotch deerhound, the male is much larger and heavier than the female;

and, as we shall see in a future chapter, the male goes on

increasing in size to an unusually late period of life, which,

according to rule, will account for his increased size being

transmitted to his male offspring alone. On the other hand, the

tortoise-shell colour, which is confined to female cats, is quite

distinct at birth, and this case violates the rule. There is a breed

of pigeons in which the males alone are streaked with black, and the

streaks can be detected even in the nestlings; but they become more

conspicuous at each successive moult, so that this case partly opposes

and partly supports the rule. With the English carrier and pouter

pigeons, the full development of the wattle and the crop occurs rather

late in life, and conformably with the rule, these characters are

transmitted in full perfection to the males alone. The following cases

perhaps come within the class previously alluded to, in which both

sexes have varied in the same manner at a rather late period of

life, and have consequently transferred their new characters to both

sexes at a corresponding late period; and if so, these cases are not

opposed to our rule:- there exist sub-breeds of the pigeon,

described by Neumeister,* in which both sexes change their colour

during two or three moults (as is likewise the case with the almond

tumbler); nevertheless, these changes, though occurring rather late in

life, are common to both sexes. One variety of the canary-bird, namely

the London Prize, offers a nearly analogous case.



  * Das Ganze der Taubenzucht, 1837, ss. 21, 24. For the case of the

streaked pigeons, see Dr. Chapuis, Le Pigeon Voyageur Belge, 1855,

p. 87.



  With the breeds of the fowl the inheritance of various characters by

one or both sexes, seems generally determined by the period at which

such characters are developed. Thus in all the many breeds in which

the adult male differs greatly in colour from the female, as well as

from the wild parent-species, he differs also from the young male,

so that the newly-acquired characters must have appeared at a rather

late period of life. On the other hand, in most of the breeds in which

the two sexes resemble each other, the young are coloured in nearly

the same manner as their parents, and this renders it probable that

their colours first appeared early in life. We have instances of

this fact in all black and white breeds, in which the young and old of

both sexes are alike; nor can it be maintained that there is something

peculiar in a black or white plumage, which leads to its

transference to both sexes; for the males alone of many natural

species are either black or white, the females being differently

coloured. With the so-called cuckoo sub-breeds of the fowl, in which

the feathers are transversely pencilled with dark stripes, both

sexes and the chickens are coloured in nearly the same manner. The

laced plumage of the Sebright bantam is the same in both sexes, and in

the young chickens the wing-feathers are distinctly, though

imperfectly laced. Spangled Hamburgs, however, offer a partial

exception; for the two sexes, though not quite alike, resemble each

other more closely than do the sexes of the aboriginal parent-species;

yet they acquire their characteristic plumage late in life, for the

chickens are distinctly pencilled. With respect to other characters

beside colour, in the wild-parent species and in most of the

domestic breeds, the males alone possess a well-developed comb; but in

the young of the Spanish fowl it is largely developed at a very

early age, and, in accordance with this early development in the male,

it is of unusual size in the adult female. In the game breeds

pugnacity is developed at a wonderfully early age, of which curious

proofs could be given; and this character is transmitted to both

sexes, so that the hens, from their extreme pugnacity, are now

generally exhibited in separate pens. With the Polish breeds the

bony protuberance of the skull which supports the crest is partially

developed even before the chickens are hatched, and the crest itself

soon begins to grow, though at first feebly;* and in this breed the

adults of both sexes are characterised by a great bony protuberance

and an immense crest.



  * For full particulars and references on all these points respecting

the several breeds of the fowl, see Variation of Animals and Plants

under Domestication, vol. i., pp. 250, 256. In regard to the higher

animals, the sexual differences which have arisen under

domestication are described in the same work under the head of each

species.



  Finally, from what we have now seen of the relation which exists

in many natural species and domesticated races, between the period

of the development of their characters and the manner of their

transmission- for example, the striking fact of the early growth of

the horns in the reindeer, in which both sexes bear horns, in

comparison with their much later growth in the other species in

which the male alone bears horns- we may conclude that one, though not

the sole cause of characters being exclusively inherited by one sex,

is their development at a late age. And secondly, that one, though

apparently a less efficient cause of characters being inherited by

both sexes, is their development at an early age, whilst the sexes

differ but little in constitution. It appears, however, that some

difference must exist between the sexes even during a very early

embryonic period, for characters developed at this age not rarely

become attached to one sex.



  Summary and concluding remarks.- From the foregoing discussion on

the various laws of inheritance, we learn that the characters of the

parents often, or even generally, tend to become developed in the

offspring of the same sex, at the same age, and periodically at the

same season of the year, in which they first appeared in the

parents. But these rules, owing to unknown causes, are far from

being fired. Hence during the modification of a species, the

successive changes may readily be transmitted in different ways;

some to one sex, and some to both; some to the offspring at one age,

and some to the offspring at all ages. Not only are the laws of

inheritance extremely complex, but so are the causes which induce

and govern variability. The variations thus induced are preserved

and accumulated by sexual selection, which is in itself an extremely

complex affair, depending, as it does, on the ardour in love, the

courage, and the rivalry of the males, as well as on the powers of

perception, the taste, and will of the female. Sexual selection will

also be largely dominated by natural selection tending towards the

general welfare of the species. Hence the manner in which the

individuals of either or both sexes have been affected through

sexual selection cannot fail to be complex in the highest degree.

  When variations occur late in life in one sex, and are transmitted

to the same sex at the same age, the other sex and the young are

left unmodified. When they occur late in life, but are transmitted

to both sexes at the same age, the young alone are left unmodified.

Variations, however, may occur at any period of life in one sex or

in both, and be transmitted to both sexes at all ages, and then all

the individuals of the species are similarly modified. In the

following chapters it will be seen that all these cases frequently

occur in nature.

  Sexual selection can never act on any animal before the age for

reproduction arrives. From the great eagerness of the male it has

generally acted on this sex and not on the females. The males have

thus become provided with weapons for fighting with their rivals, with

organs for discovering and securely holding the female, and for

exciting or charming her. When the sexes differ in these respects,

it is also, as we have seen, an extremely general law that the adult

male differs more or less from the young male; and we may conclude

from this fact that the successive variations, by which the adult male

became modified, did not generally occur much before the age for

reproduction. Whenever some or many of the variations occurred early

in life, the young males would partake more or less of the

characters of the adult males; and differences of this kind between

the old and young males may be observed in many species of animals.

  It is probable that young male animals have often tended to vary

in a manner which would not only have been of no use to them at an

early age, but would have been actually injurious- as by acquiring

bright colours, which would render them conspicuous to their

enemies, or by acquiring structures, such as great horns, which

would expend much vital force in their development. Variations of this

kind occurring in the young males would almost certainly be eliminated

through natural selection. With the adult and experienced males, on

the other hand, the advantages derived from the acquisition of such

characters, would more than counterbalance some exposure to danger,

and some loss of vital force.

  As variations which give to the male a better chance of conquering

other males, or of finding, securing, or charming the opposite sex,

would, if they happened to arise in the female, be of no service to

her, they would not be preserved in her through sexual selection. We

have also good evidence with domesticated animals, that variations

of all kinds are, if not carefully selected, soon lost through

inter-crossing and accidental deaths. Consequently in a state of

nature, if variations of the above kind chanced to arise in the female

line, and be transmitted exclusively in this line, they would be

extremely liable to be lost. If, however, the females varied and

transmitted their newly acquired characters to their offspring of both

sexes, the characters which were advantageous to the males would be

preserved by them through sexual selection, and the two sexes would in

consequence be modified in the same manner, although such characters

were of no use to the females: but I shall hereafter have to recur

to these more intricate contingencies. Lastly, the females may

acquire, and apparently have often acquired by transference,

characters from the male sex.

  As variations occurring later in life, and transmitted to one sex

alone, have incessantly been taken advantage of and accumulated

through sexual selection in relation to the reproduction of the

species; therefore it appears, at first sight, an unaccountable fact

that similar variations have not frequently been accumulated through

natural selection, in relation to the ordinary habits of life. If this

had occurred, the two sexes would often have been differently

modified, for the sake, for instance, of capturing prey or of escaping

from danger. Differences of this kind between the two sexes do

occasionally occur, especially in the lower classes. But this

implies that the two sexes follow different habits in their

struggles for existence, which is a rare circumstance with the

higher animals. The case, however, is widely different with the

reproductive functions, in which respect the sexes necessarily differ.

For variations in structure which are related to these functions, have

often proved of value to one sex, and from having arisen at a late

period of life, have been transmitted to one sex alone; and such

variations, thus preserved and transmitted, have given rise to

secondary sexual characters.

  In the following chapters, I shall treat of the secondary sexual

characters in animals of all classes, and shall endeavour in each case

to apply the principles explained in the present chapter. The lowest

classes will detain us for a very short time, but the higher

animals, especially birds, must be treated at considerable length.

It should be borne in mind that for reasons already assigned, I intend

to give only a few illustrative instances of the innumerable

structures by the aid of which the male finds the female, or, when

found, holds her. On the other hand, all structures and instincts by

the aid of which the male conquers other males, and by which he

allures or excites the female, will be fully discussed, as these are

in many ways the most interesting.



  Supplement on the proportional numbers of the two sexes in animals

belonging to various classes.



  As no one, as far as I can discover, has paid attention to the

relative numbers of the two sexes throughout the animal kingdom, I

will here give such materials as I have been able to collect, although

they are extremely imperfect. They consist in only a few instances

of actual enumeration, and the numbers are not very large. As the

proportions are known with certainty only in mankind, I will first

give them as a standard of comparison.



  Man.- In England during ten years (from 1857 to 1866) the average

number of children born alive yearly was 707,120, in the proportion of

104.5 males to 100 females. But in 1857 the male births throughout

England were as 105.2, and in 1865 as 104.0 to 100. Looking to

separate districts, in Buckinghamshire (where about 5000 children

are annually born) the mean proportion of male to female births,

during the whole period of the above ten years, was as 102.8 to 100;

whilst in N. Wales (where the average annual births are 12,873) it was

as high as 106.2 to 100. Taking a still smaller district, viz.,

Rutlandshire (where the annual births average only 739), in 1864 the

male births were as 114.6, and in 1862 as only 97.0 to 100; but even

in this small district the average of the 7385 births during the whole

ten years, was as 104.5 to 100: that is in the same ratio as

throughout England.* The proportions are sometimes slightly

disturbed by unknown causes; thus Prof. Faye states "that in some

districts of Norway there has been during a decennial period a

steady deficiency of boys, whilst in others the opposite condition has

existed." In France during forty-four years the male to the female

births have been as 106.2 to 100; but during this period it has

occurred five times in one department, and six times in another,

that the female births have exceeded the males. In Russia the

average proportion is as high as 108.9, and in Philadelphia in the

United States as 110.5 to 100.*(2) The average for Europe, deduced

by Bickes from about seventy million births, is 106 males to 100

females. On the other hand, with white children born at the Cape of

Good Hope, the proportion of males is so low as to fluctuate during

successive years between 90 and 99 males for every 100 females. It

is a singular fact that with Jews the proportion of male births is

decidedly larger than with Christians: thus in Prussia the

proportion is as 113, in Breslau as 114, and in Livonia as 120 to 100;

the Christian births in these countries being the same as usual, for

instance, in Livonia as 104 to 100.*(3)



  * Twenty-ninth Annual Report of the Registrar-General for 1866. In

this report (p. xii.) a special decennial table is given.

  *(2) For Norway and Russia, see abstract of Prof. Faye's researches,

in British and Foreign Medico-Chirurg. Review, April, 1867, pp. 343,

345. For France, the Annuaire pour l'An 1867, p. 213. For

Philadelphia, Dr. Stockton-Hough, Social Science Assoc., 1874. For the

Cape of Good Hope, Quetelet as quoted by Dr. H. H. Zouteveen, in the

Dutch translation of this work (vol. i., p. 417), where much

information is given on the proportion of the sexes.

  *(3) In regard to the Jews, see M. Thury, La Loi de Production des

Sexes, 1863, p. 25.



  Prof. Faye remarks that "a still greater preponderance of males

would be met with, if death struck both sexes in equal proportion in

the womb and during birth. But the fact is, that for every 100

still-born females, we have in several countries from 134.6 to 144.9

stillborn males. During the first four or five years of life, also,

more male children die than females, for example in England, during

the first year, 126 boys die for every 100 girls- a proportion which

in France is still more unfavourable."* Dr. Stockton-Hough accounts

for these facts in part by the more frequent defective development

of males than of females. We have before seen that the male sex is

more variable in structure than the female; and variations in

important organs would generally be injurious. But the size of the

body, and especially of the head, being greater in male than female

infants is another cause: for the males are thus more liable to be

injured during parturition. Consequently the still-born males are more

numerous; and, as a highly competent judge, Dr. Crichton Browne,*(2)

believes, male infants often suffer in health for some years after

birth. Owing to this excess in the death-rate of male children, both

at birth and for some time subsequently, and owing to the exposure

of grown men to various dangers, and to their tendency to emigrate,

the females in all old-settled countries, where statistical records

have been kept,*(3) are found to preponderate considerably over the

males.



  * British and Foreign Medico-Chirurg. Review, April, 1867, p. 343.

Dr. Stark also remarks (Tenth Annual Reports of Births, Deaths, &c.,

in Scotland, 1867, p. xxviii.) that "These examples may suffice to

show that, at almost every stage of life, the males in Scotland have a

greater liability to death and a higher death-rate than the females.

The fact, however, of this peculiarity being most strongly developed

at that infantile period of life when the dress, food, and general

treatment of both sexes are alike, seems to prove that the higher male

death-rate is an impressed, natural, and constitutional peculiarity

due to sex alone."

  *(2) West Riding Lunatic Asylum Reports, vol. i., 1871, p. 8. Sir J.

Simpson has proved that the head of the male infant exceeds that of

the female by 3/8ths of an inch in circumference, and by 1/8th in

transverse diameter. Quetelet has shown that woman is born smaller

than man; see Dr. Duncan, Fecundity, Fertility, and Sterility, 1871,

p. 382.

  *(3) With the savage Guaranys of Paraguay, according to the accurate

Azara (Voyages dans l'Amerique merid., tom. ii., 1809, pp. 60, 179),

the women are to the men in the proportion of 14 to 13.



  It seems at first sight a mysterious fact that in different nations,

under different conditions and climates, in Naples, Prussia,

Westphalia, Holland, France, England and the United States, the excess

of male over female births is less when they are illegitimate than

when legitimate.* This has been explained by different writers in many

different ways, as from the mothers being generally young, from the

large proportion of first pregnancies, &c. But we have seen that

male infants, from the large size of their heads, suffer more than

female infants during parturition; and as the mothers of

illegitimate children must be more liable than other women to

undergo bad labours, from various causes, such as attempts at

concealment by tight lacing, hard work, distress of mind, &c., their

male infants would proportionably suffer. And this probably is the

most efficient of all the causes of the proportion of males to females

born alive being less amongst illegitimate children than amongst the

legitimate. With most animals the greater size of the adult male

than of the female, is due to the stronger males having conquered

the weaker in their struggles for the possession of the females, and

no doubt it is owing to this fact that the two sexes of at least

some animals differ in size at birth. Thus we have the curious fact

that we may attribute the more frequent deaths of male than female

infants, especially amongst the illegitimate, at least in part to

sexual selection.



  * Babbage, Edinburgh Journal of Science, 1829, vol. i., p. 88;

also p. 90, on still-born children. On illegitimate children in

England, see Report of Registrar-General for 1866, p. xv.



  It has often been supposed that the relative age of the two

parents determine the sex of the offspring; and Prof. Leuckart* has

advanced what he considers sufficient evidence, with respect to man

and certain domesticated animals, that this is one important though

not the sole factor in the result. So again the period of impregnation

relatively to the state of the female has been thought by some to be

the efficient cause; but recent observations discountenance this

belief. According to Dr. Stockton-Hough,*(2) the season of the year,

the poverty or wealth of the parents, residence in the country or in

cities, the crossing of foreign immigrants, &c., all influence the

proportion of the sexes. With mankind, polygamy has also been supposed

to lead to the birth of a greater proportion of female infants; but

Dr. J. Campbell*(3) carefully attended to this subject in the harems

of Siam, and concludes that the proportion of male to female births is

the same as from monogamous unions. Hardly any animal has been

rendered so highly polygamous as the English race-horse, and we

shall immediately see that his male and female offspring are almost

exactly equal in number. I will now give the facts which I have

collected with respect to the proportional numbers of the sexes of

various animals; and will then briefly discuss how far selection has

come into play in determining the result.



  * Leuckart, in Wagner's Handworterbuch der Phys., B. iv., 1853, s.

774.

  *(2) Social Science Association of Philadelphia, 1874.

  *(3) Anthropological Review, April, 1870, p. cviii.



  Horses.- Mr. Tegetmeier has been so kind as to tabulate for me

from the Racing Calendar the births of race-horses during a period

of twenty-one years, viz., from 1846 to 1867; 1849 being omitted, as

no returns were that year published. The total births were 25,560,*

consisting of 12,763 males and 12,797 females, or in the proportion of

99.7 males to 100 females. As these numbers are tolerably large, and

as they are drawn from all parts of England, during several years,

we may with much confidence conclude that with the domestic horse,

or at least with the race-horse, the two sexes are produced in

almost equal numbers. The fluctuations in the proportions during

successive years are closely like those which occur with mankind, when

a small and thinly-populated area is considered; thus in 1856 the male

horses were as 107.1, and in 1867 as only 92.6 to 100 females. In

the tabulated returns the proportions vary in cycles, for the males

exceeded the females during six successive years; and the females

exceeded the males during two periods each of four years; this,

however, may be accidental; at least I can detect nothing of the

kind with man in the decennial table in the Registrar's Report for

1866.



  * During eleven years a record was kept of the number of mares which

proved barren or prematurely slipped their foals; and it deserves

notice, as shewing how infertile these highly-nurtured and rather

closely-interbred animals have become, that not far from one-third

of the mares failed to produce living foals. Thus during 1866, 809

male colts and 816 female colts were born, and 743 mares failed to

produce offspring. During 1867, 836 males and 902 females were born,

and 794 mares failed.



  Dogs.- During a period of twelve years, from 1857 to 1868, the

births of a large number of greyhounds, throughout England, were

sent to the Field newspaper; and I am again indebted to Mr. Tegetmeier

for carefully tabulating the results. The recorded births were 6878,

consisting of 3605 males and 3273 females, that is, in the

proportion of 110.1 males to 100 females. The greatest fluctuations

occurred in 1864, when the proportion was as 95.3 males, and in

1867, as 116.3 males to 100 females. The above average proportion of

110.1 to 100 is probably nearly correct in the case of the

greyhound, but whether it would hold with other domesticated breeds is

in some degree doubtful. Mr. Cupples has enquired from several great

breeders of dogs, and finds that all without exception believe that

females are produced in excess; but he suggests that this belief may

have arisen from females being less valued, and from the consequent

disappointment producing a stronger impression on the mind.

  Sheep.- The sexes of sheep are not ascertained by agriculturists

until several months after birth, at the period when the males are

castrated; so that the following returns do not give the proportions

at birth. Moreover, I find that several great breeders in Scotland,

who annually raise some thousand sheep, are firmly convinced that a

larger proportion of males than of females die during the first year

or two. Therefore the proportion of males would be somewhat larger

at birth than at the age of castration. This is a remarkable

coincidence with what, as we have seen, occurs with mankind, and

both cases probably depend on the same cause. I have received

returns from four gentlemen in England who have bred lowland sheep,

chiefly Leicesters, during the last ten to sixteen years; they

amount altogether to 8965 births, consisting of 4407 males and 4558

females; that is in the proportion of 96.7 males to 100 females.

With respect to Cheviot and black-faced sheep bred in Scotland, I have

received returns from six breeders, two of them on a large scale,

chiefly for the years 1867-1869, but some of the returns extend back

to 1862. The total number recorded amounts to 50,685, consisting of

25,071 males and 25,614 females or in the proportion of 97.9 males

to 100 females. If we take the English and Scotch returns together,

the total number amounts to 59,650, consisting of 29,478 males and

30,172 females, or as 97.7 to 100. So that with sheep at the age of

castration the females are certainly in excess of the males, but

probably this would not hold good at birth.*



  *I am much indebted to Mr. Cupples for having procured for me the

above returns from Scotland, as well as some of the following

returns on cattle. Mr. R. Elliot, of Laighwood, first called my

attention to the premature deaths of the males,- a statement

subsequently confirmed by Mr. Aitchison and others. To this latter

gentleman, and to Mr. Payan, I owe my thanks for large returns as to

sheep.



  Of Cattle I have received returns from nine gentlemen of 982 births,

too few to be trusted; these consisted of 477 bull-calves and 505

cow-calves; i.e., in the proportion of 94.4 males to 100 females.

The Rev. W. D. Fox informs me that in 1867 out of 34 calves born on

a farm in Derbyshire only one was a bull. Mr. Harrison Weir has

enquired from several breeders of Pigs, and most of them estimate

the male to the female births as about 7 to 6. This same gentleman has

bred rabbits for many years, and has noticed that a far greater number

of bucks are produced than does. But estimations are of little value.

  Of Mammalia in a state of nature I have been able to learn very

little. In regard to the common rat, I have received conflicting

statements. Mr. R. Elliot, of Laighwood, informs me that a rat-catcher

assured him that he had always found the males in great excess, even

with the young in the nest. In consequence of this, Mr. Elliot himself

subsequently examined some hundred old ones, and found the statement

true. Mr. F. Buckland has bred a large number of white rats, and he

also believes that the males greatly exceed the females. In regard

to moles, it is said that "the males are much more numerous than the

females":* and as the catching of these animals is a special

occupation, the statement may perhaps be trusted. Sir A. Smith, in

describing an antelope of S. Africa*(2) (Kobus ellipsiprymnus),

remarks, that in the herds of this and other species, the males are

few in number compared with the females: the natives believe that they

are born in this proportion; others believe that the younger males are

expelled from the herds, and Sir A. Smith says, that though he has

himself never seen herds consisting of young males alone, others

affirm that this does occur. It appears probable that the young when

expelled from the herd, would often fall a prey to the many beasts

of prey of the country.



  * Bell, History of British Quadrupeds, p. 100.

  *(2) Illustrations of the Zoology of S. Africa, 1849, pl. 29.



                          BIRDS.



  With respect to the Fowl, I have received only one account,

namely, that out of 1001 chickens of a highly-bred stock of Cochins,

reared during eight years by Mr. Stretch, 487 proved males and 514

females; i.e., as 94.7 to 100. In regard to domestic pigeons there

is good evidence either that the males are produced in excess, or that

they live longer; for these birds invariably pair, and single males,

as Mr. Tegetmeier informs me, can always be purchased cheaper than

females. Usually the two birds reared from the two eggs laid in the

same nest are a male and a female; but Mr. Harrison Weir, who has been

so large a breeder, says that he has often bred two cocks from the

same nest, and seldom two hens; moreover, the hen is generally the

weaker of the two, and more liable to perish.

  With respect to birds in a state of nature, Mr. Gould and others*

are convinced that the males are generally the more numerous; and as

the young males of many species resemble the females, the latter would

naturally appear to be the more numerous. Large numbers of pheasants

are reared by Mr. Baker of Leadenhall from eggs laid by wild birds,

and he informs Mr. Jenner Weir that four or five males to one female

are generally produced. An experienced observer remarks,*(2) that in

Scandinavia the broods of the capercailzie and black-cock contain more

males than females; and that with the Dal-ripa (a kind of ptarmigan)

more males than females attend the leks or places of courtship; but

this latter circumstance is accounted for by some observers by a

greater number of hen birds being killed by vermin. From various facts

given by White of Selborne,*(3) it seems clear that the males of the

partridge must be in considerable excess in the south of England;

and I have been assured that this is the case in Scotland. Mr. Weir on

enquiring from the dealers, who receive at certain seasons large

numbers of ruffs (Machetes pugnax), was told that the males are much

the more numerous. This same naturalist has also enquired for me

from the birdcatchers, who annually catch an astonishing number of

various small species alive for the London market, and he was

unhesitatingly answered by an old and trustworthy man, that with the

chaffinch the males are in large excess: he thought as high as 2 males

to 1 female, or at least as high as 5 to 3.*(4) The males of the

blackbird, he likewise maintained, were by far the more numerous,

whether caught by traps or by netting at night. These statements may

apparently be trusted, because this same man said that the sexes are

about equal with the lark, the twite (Linaria montana), and goldfinch.

On the other hand, he is certain that with the common linnet, the

females preponderate greatly, but unequally during different years;

during some years he has found the females to the males as four to

one. It should, however, be borne in mind, that the chief season for

catching birds does not begin till September, so that with some

species partial migrations may have begun, and the flocks at this

period often consist of hens alone. Mr. Salvin paid particular

attention to the sexes of the humming-birds in Central America, and is

convinced that with most of the species the males are in excess;

thus one year he procured 204 specimens belonging to ten species,

and these consisted of 166 males and of only 38 females. With two

other species the females were in excess: but the proportions

apparently vary either during different seasons or in different

localities; for on one occasion the males of Campylopterus

hemileucurus were to the females as 5 to 2, and on another

occasion*(5) in exactly the reversed ratio. As bearing on this

latter point, I may add, that Mr. Powys found in Corfu and Epirus

the sexes of the chaffinch keeping apart, and "the females by far

the most numerous"; whilst in Palestine Mr. Tristram found "the male

flocks appearing greatly to exceed the female in number."*(6) So again

with the Quiscalus major, Mr. G. Taylor says, that in Florida there

were "very few females in proportion to the males,"*(7) whilst in

Honduras the proportion was the other way, the species there having

the character of a polygamist.



  * Brehm (Illustriertes Thierleben, B. iv., s. 990) comes to the same

conclusion.

  *(2) On the authority of L. Lloyd, Game Birds of Sweden, 1867, pp.

12, 132.

  *(3) Nat. Hist. of Selborne, letter xxix., ed. of 1825, vol. i.,

p. 139.

  *(4) Mr. Jenner Weir received similar information, on making

enquiries during the following year. To shew the number of living

chaffinches caught, I may mention that in 1869 there was a match

between two experts, and one man caught in a day 62, and another 40,

male chaffinches. The greatest number ever caught by one man in a

single day was 70.

  *(5) Ibis, vol. ii. p. 260, as quoted in Gould's Trochilidae,

1861, p. 52. For the foregoing proportions, I am indebted to Mr.

Salvin for a table of his results.

  *(6) Ibis, 1860, p. 137; and 1867, p. 369.

  *(7) Ibis, 1862, p. 187.



                           FISH.



  With fish the proportional numbers of the sexes can be ascertained

only by catching them in the adult or nearly adult state; and there

are many difficulties in arriving at any just conclusion.* Infertile

females might readily be mistaken for males, as Dr. Gunther has

remarked to me in regard to trout. With some species the males are

believed to die soon after fertilising the ova. With many species

the males are of much smaller size than the females, so that a large

number of males would escape from the same net by which the females

were caught. M. Carbonnier,*(2) who has especially attended to the

natural history of the pike (Esox lucius), states that many males,

owing to their small size, are devoured by the larger females; and

he believes that the males of almost all fish are exposed from this

same cause to greater danger than the females. Nevertheless, in the

few cases in which the proportional numbers have been actually

observed, the males appear to be largely in excess. Thus Mr. R. Buist,

the superintendent of the Stormontfield experiments, says that in

1865, out of 70 salmon first landed for the purpose of obtaining the

ova, upwards of 60 were males. In 1867 he again "calls attention to

the vast disproportion of the males to the females. We had at the

outset at least ten males to one female." Afterwards females

sufficient for obtaining ova were procured. He adds, "from the great

proportion of the males, they are constantly fighting and tearing each

other on the spawning-beds."*(3) This disproportion, no doubt, can

be accounted for in part, but whether wholly is doubtful, by the males

ascending the rivers before the females. Mr. F. Buckland remarks in

regard to trout, that "it is a curious fact that the males

preponderate very largely in number over the females. It invariably

happens that when the first rush of fish is made to the net, there

will be at least seven or eight males to one female found captive. I

cannot quite account for this; either the males are more numerous than

the females, or the latter seek safety by concealment rather than

flight." He then adds, that by carefully searching the banks

sufficient females for obtaining ova can be found.*(4) Mr. H. Lee

informs me that out of 212 trout taken for this purpose in Lord

Portsmouth's park, 150 were males and 62 females.



  * Leuckart quotes Bloch (Wagner, Handworterbuch der Phys., B. iv.,

1853, s. 775), that with fish there are twice as many males as

females.

  *(2) Quoted in the Farmer, March 18, 1869, p. 369.

  *(3) The Stormontfield Piscicultural Experiments, 1866, p. 23. The

Field newspaper, June 29, 1867.

  *(4) Land and Water, 1868, p. 41.



  The males of the Cyprinidae likewise seem to be in excess; but

several members of this family, viz., the carp, tench, bream and

minnow, appear regularly to follow the practice, rare in the animal

kingdom, of polyandry; for the female whilst spawning is always

attended by two males, one on each side, and in the case of the

bream by three or four males. This fact is so well known, that it is

always recommended to stock a pond with two male tenches to one

female, or at least with three males to two females. With the

minnow, an excellent observer states, that on the spawning-beds the

males are ten times as numerous as the females; when a female comes

amongst the males, "she is immediately pressed closely by a male on

each side; and when they have been in that situation for a time, are

superseded by other two males."*



  * Yarrell, Hist. British Fishes, vol. i., 1826, p. 307; on the

Cyprinus carpio, p. 331; on the Tinca vulgaris, p. 331; on the Abramis

brama, p. 336. See, for the minnow (Leuciscus phoxinus), Loudon's

Magazine of Natural History, vol. v., 1832, p. 682.



                         INSECTS.



  In this great class, the Lepidoptera almost alone affords means

for judging of the proportional numbers of the sexes; for they have

been collected with special care by many good observers, and have been

largely bred from the egg or caterpillar state. I had hoped that

some breeders of silk-moths might have kept an exact record, but after

writing to France and Italy, and consulting various treatises, I

cannot find that this has ever been done. The general opinion

appears to be that the sexes are nearly equal, but in Italy, as I hear

from Professor Canestrini, many breeders are convinced that the

females are produced in excess. This same naturalist, however, informs

me, that in the two yearly broods of the ailanthus silk-moth (Bombyx

cynthia), the males greatly preponderate in the first, whilst in the

second the two sexes are nearly equal, or the females rather in

excess.

  In regard to butterflies in a state of nature, several observers

have been much struck by the apparently enormous preponderance of

the males.* Thus Mr. Bates,*(2) in speaking of several species,

about a hundred in number, which inhabit the upper Amazons, says

that the males are much more numerous than the females, even in the

proportion of a hundred to one. In North America, Edwards, who had

great experience, estimates in the genus Papilio the males to the

females as four to one; and Mr. Walsh, who informs me of this

statement, says that with P. turnus this is certainly the case. In

South Africa, Mr. R. Trimen found the males in excess in 19

species;*(3) and in one of these, which swarms in open places, he

estimated the number of males as fifty to one female. With another

species, in which the males are numerous in certain localities, he

collected only five females during seven years. In the island of

Bourbon, M. Maillard states that the males of one species of Papilio

are twenty times as numerous as the females.*(4) Mr. Trimen informs me

that as far as he has himself seen, or heard from others, it is rare

for the females of any butterfly to exceed the males in number; but

three South African species perhaps offer an exception. Mr.

Wallace*(5) states that the females of Ornithoptera croesus, in the

Malay Archipelago, are more common and more easily caught than the

males; but this is a rare butterfly. I may here add, that in

Hyperythra, a genus of moths, Guenee says, that from four to five

females are sent in collections from India for one male.



  * Leuckart quotes Meinecke (Wagner, Handworterbuch der Phys., B.

iv., 1853, s. 775) that the males of butterflies are three or four

times as numerous as the females.

  *(2) The Naturalist on the Amazons, vol. ii., 1863, pp. 228, 347.

  *(3) Four of these cases are given by Mr. Trimen in his

Rhopalocera Africae Australis.

  *(4) Quoted by Trimen, Transactions of the Ent. Society, vol. v.,

part iv., 1866, p. 330.

  *(5) Transactions, Linnean Society, vol. xxv., p. 37.



  When this subject of the proportional numbers of the sexes of

insects was brought before the Entomological Society,* it was

generally admitted that the males of most Lepidoptera, in the adult or

imago state, are caught in greater numbers than the females: but

this fact was attributed by various observers to the more retiring

habits of the females, and to the males emerging earlier from the

cocoon. This latter circumstance is well known to occur with most

Lepidoptera, as well as with other insects. So that, as M. Personnat

remarks, the males of the domesticated Bombyx yamamai., are useless at

the beginning of the season, and the females at the end, from the want

of mates.*(2) I cannot, however, persuade myself that these causes

suffice to explain the great excess of males, in the above cases of

certain butterflies which are extremely common in their native

countries. Mr. Stainton, who has paid very close attention during many

years to the smaller moths, informs me that when he collected them

in the imago state, he thought that the males were ten times as

numerous as the females, but that since he has reared them on a

large scale from the caterpillar state, he is convinced that the

females are the more numerous. Several entomologists concur in this

view. Mr. Doubleday, however, and some others, take an opposite

view, and are convinced that they have reared from the eggs and

caterpillars a larger proportion of males than of females.



  * Proceedings, Entomological Society, Feb. 17, 1868.

  *(2) Quoted by Dr. Wallace in Proceedings, Entomological Society,

3rd series, vol. v., 1867, p. 487.



  Besides the more active habits of the males, their earlier emergence

from the cocoon, and in some cases their frequenting more open

stations, other causes may be assigned for an apparent or real

difference in the proportional numbers of the sexes of Lepidoptera,

when captured in the imago state, and when reared from the egg or

caterpillar state. I hear from Professor Canestrini, that it is

believed by many breeders in Italy, that the female caterpillar of the

silk-moth suffers more from the recent disease than the male; and

Dr. Staudinger informs me that in rearing Lepidoptera more females die

in the cocoon than males. With many species the female caterpillar

is larger than the male, and a collector would naturally choose the

finest specimens, and thus unintentionally collect a larger number

of females. Three collectors have told me that this was their

practice; but Dr. Wallace is sure that most collectors take all the

specimens which they can find of the rarer kinds, which alone are

worth the trouble of rearing. Birds when surrounded by caterpillars

would probably devour the largest; and Professor Canestrini informs me

that in Italy some breeders believe, though on insufficient

evidence, that in the first broods of the ailanthus silkmoth, the

wasps destroy a larger number of the female than of the male

caterpillars. Dr. Wallace further remarks that female caterpillars,

from being larger than the males, require more time for their

development, and consume more food and moisture: and thus they would

be exposed during a longer time to danger from ichneumons, birds, &c.,

and in times of scarcity would perish in greater numbers. Hence it

appears quite possible that in a state of nature, fewer female

Lepidoptera may reach maturity than males; and for our special

object we are concerned with their relative numbers at maturity,

when the sexes are ready to propagate their kind.

  The manner in which the males of certain moths congregate in

extraordinary numbers round a single female, apparently indicates a

great excess of males, though this fact may perhaps be accounted for

by the earlier emergence of the males from their cocoons. Mr. Stainton

informs me that from twelve to twenty males, may often be seen

congregated round a female Elachista rufocinerea. It is well known

that if a virgin Lasiocampa quercus or Saturnia carpini be exposed

in a cage, vast numbers of males collect round her, and if confined in

a room will even come down the chimney to her. Mr. Doubleday

believes that he has seen from fifty to a hundred males of both

these species attracted in the course of a single day by a female in

confinement. In the Isle of Wight Mr. Trimen exposed a box in which

a female of the Lasiocampa had been confined on the previous day,

and five males soon endeavored to gain admittance. In Australia, Mr.

Verreaux, having placed the female of a small Bombyx in a box in his

pocket, was followed by a crowd of males, so that about 200 entered

the house with him.*



  * Blanchard, Metamorphoses, Moeurs des Insectes, 1868, pp. 225-226.



  Mr. Doubleday has called my attention to M. Staudinger's* list of

Lepidoptera, which gives the prices of the males and females of 300

species or well-marked varieties of butterflies (Rhopalocera). The

prices for both sexes of the very common species are of course the

same; but in 114 of the rarer species they differ; the males being

in all cases, excepting one, the cheaper. On an average of the

prices of the 113 species, the price of the male to that of the female

is as 100 to 149; and this apparently indicates that inversely the

males exceed the females in the same proportion. About 2000 species or

varieties of moths (Heterocera) are catalogued, those with wingless

females being here excluded on account of the difference in habits

between the two sexes: of these 2000 species, 141 differ in price

according to sex, the males of 130 being cheaper, and those of only 11

being dearer than the females. The average price of the males of the

130 species, to that of the females, is as 100 to 143. With respect to

the butterflies in this priced list, Mr. Doubleday thinks (and no

man in England has had more experience), that there is nothing in

the habits of the species which can account for the difference in

the prices of the two sexes, and that it can be accounted for only

by an excess in the number of the males. But I am bound to add that

Dr. Staudinger informs me, that he is himself of a different

opinion. He thinks that the less active habits of the females and

the earlier emergence of the males will account for his collectors

securing a larger number of males than of females, and consequently

for the lower prices of the former. With respect to specimens reared

from the caterpillar-state, Dr. Staudinger believes, as previously

stated, that a greater number of females than of males die whilst

confined to the cocoons. He adds that with certain species one sex

seems to preponderate over the other during certain years.



  * Lepidopteren-Doubletten Liste, Berlin, No. x., 1866.



  Of direct observations on the sexes of Lepidoptera, reared either

from eggs or caterpillars, I have received only the few following

cases:*



  * See following table.



  So that in these eight lots of cocoons and eggs, males were produced

in excess. Taken together the proportion of males is as 122.7 to 100

females. But the numbers are hardly large enough to be trustworthy.

  On the whole, from these various sources of evidence, all pointing

in the same direction, I infer that with most species of

Lepidoptera, the mature males generally exceed the females in

number, whatever the proportions may be at their first emergence

from the egg.



                                                    Males   Females

  The Rev. J. Hellins* of Exeter reared, during

    1868, imagos of 73 species, which

    consisted of                                     153       137

  Mr. Albert Jones of Eltham reared, during

    1868, imagos of 9 species, which

    consisted of                                     159       126



  During 1869 he reared imagoes from 4 species

    consisting of                                    114       112



  Mr. Buckler of Emsworth, Hants, during 1869,

    reared imagos from 74 species,

    consisting of                                    180       169



  Dr. Wallace of Colchester reared from one

    brood of Bombyx cynthia                           52        48



  Dr. Wallace raised, from cocoons of Bombyx

    pernyi sent from China, during 1869              224       123



  Dr. Wallace raised, during 1868 and 1869, from

    two lots of cocoons of Bombyx yamamai             52        46



                                           Total     934       761



  * This naturalist has been so kind as to send me some results from

former years, in which the females seemed to preponderate; but so many

of the figures were estimates, that I found it impossible to

tabulate them



  With reference to the other orders of insects, I have been able to

collect very little reliable information. With the stag-beetle

(Lucanus cervus) "the males appear to be much more numerous than the

females"; but when, as Cornelius remarked during 1867, an unusal

number of these beetles appeared in one part of Germany, the females

appeared to exceed the males as six to one. With one of the

Elateridae, the males are said to be much more numerous than the

females, and "two or three are often found united with one female;* so

that here polyandry seems to prevail." With Siagonium (Staphylinidae),

in which the males are furnished with horns, "the females are far more

numerous than the opposite sex." Mr. Janson stated at the

Entomological Society that the females of the bark feeding Tomicus

villosus are so common as to be a plague, whilst the males are so rare

as to be hardly known.



  * Gunther's Record of Zoological Literature, 1867, p. 260. On the

excess of female Lucanus, ibid, p. 250. On the males of Lucanus in

England, Westwood, Modern Classification of Insects, vol. i., p.

187. On the Siagonium, ibid., p. 172.



  It is hardly worthwhile saying anything about the proportion of

the sexes in certain species and even groups of insects, for the males

are unknown or very rare, and the females are parthenogenetic, that

is, fertile without sexual union; examples of this are afforded by

several of the Cynipidae.* In all the gall-making Cynipidae known to

Mr. Walsh, the females are four or five times as numerous as the

males; and so it is, as he informs me, with the gall-making

Cecidomyiidae (Diptera). With some common species of saw-flies

(Tenthredinae) Mr. F. Smith has reared hundreds of specimens from

larvae of all sizes, but has never reared a single male; on the

other hand, Curtis says,*(2) that with certain species (Athalia), bred

by him, the males were to the females as six to one; whilst exactly

the reverse occurred with the mature insects of the same species

caught in the fields. In the family of bees, Hermann Muller,*(3)

collected a large number of specimens of many species, and reared

others from the cocoons, and counted the sexes. He found that the

males of some species greatly exceeded the females in number; in

others the reverse occurred; and in others the two sexes were nearly

equal. But as in most cases the males emerge from the cocoons before

the females, they are at the commencement of the breeding-season

practically in excess. Muller also observed that the relative number

of the two sexes in some species differed much in different

localities. But as H. Muller has himself remarked to me, these remarks

must be received with some caution, as one sex might more easily

escape observation than the other. Thus his brother Fritz Muller has

noticed in Brazil that the two sexes of the same species of bee

sometimes frequent different kinds of flowers. With respect to the

Orthoptera, I know hardly anything about the relative number of the

sexes: Korte,*(4) however, says that out of 500 locusts which he

examined, the males were to the females as five to six. With the

Neuroptera, Mr. Walsh states that in many, but by no means in all

the species of the odonatous group, there is a great overplus of

males: in the genus Hetaerina, also, the males are generally at

least four times as numerous as the females. In certain species in the

genus Gomphus the males are equally in excess, whilst in two other

species, the females the are twice or thrice as numerous as the males.

In some European species of Psocus thousands of females may be

collected without a single male, whilst with other species of the same

genus both sexes are common.*(5) In England, Mr. MacLachlan has

captured hundreds of the female Apatania muliebris, but has never seen

the male; and of Boreus hyemalis only four or five males have been

seen here.*(6) With most of these species (excepting the Tenthredinae)

there is at present no evidence that the females are subject to

parthenogenesis; and thus we see how ignorant we are of the causes

of the apparent discrepancy in the proportion of the two sexes.



  * Walsh in the American Entomologist, vol. i., 1869, p. 103. F.

Smith, Record of Zoological Lit, 1867, p. 328.

  *(2) Farm Insects, pp. 45-46.

  *(3) "Anwendung der Darwin'schen Lehre," Verh. d. n. Jahrg., xxiv.

  *(4) Die Strich. Zug oder Wanderheuschrecke, 1828, p. 20.

  *(5) "Observations on N. American Neuroptera," by H. Hagen and B. D.

Walsh, Proceedings, Ent. Soc. Philadelphia, Oct., 1863, pp. 168,

223, 239.

  *(6) Proceedings, Ent. Soc. London, Feb. 17, 1868.



  In the other classes of the Articulata I have been able to collect

still less information. With spiders, Mr. Blackwall, who has carefully

attended to this class during many years, writes to me that the

males from their more erratic habits are more commonly seen, and

therefore appear more numerous. This is actually the case with a few

species; but he mentions several species in six genera, in which the

females appear to be much more numerous than the males.* The small

size of the males in comparison with the females (a peculiarity

which is sometimes carried to an extreme degree), and their widely

different appearance, may account in some instances for their rarity

in collections.*(2)

8888

  * Another great authority with respect to this class, Prof.

Thorell of Upsala (On European Spiders, 1869-70, part i., p. 205),

speaks as if female spiders were generally commoner than the males.

  *(2) See, on this subject, Mr. O. P. Cambridge, as quoted in

Quarterly Journal of Science, 1868, page 429.



  Some of the lower crustaceans are able to propagate their kind

sexually, and this will account for the extreme rarity of the males;

thus von Siebold* carefully examined no less than 13,000 specimens

of Apus from twenty-one localities, and amongst these he found only

319 males. With some other forms (as Tanais and Cypris), as Fritz

Muller informs me, there is reason to believe that the males are

much shorter-lived than the females; and this would explain their

scarcity, supposing the two sexes to be at first equal in number. On

the other hand, Muller has invariably taken far more males than

females of the Diastylidae and of Cypridina on the shores of Brazil:

thus with a species in the latter genus, 63 specimens caught the

same day included 57 males; but he suggests that this preponderance

may be due to some unknown difference in the habits of the two

sexes. With one of the higher Brazilian crabs, namely a Gelasimus,

Fritz Muller found the males to be more numerous than the females.

According to the large experience of Mr. C. Spence Bate, the reverse

seems to be the case with six common British crabs, the names of which

he has given me.



  * Beitrage zur Parthenogenesis, p. 174.



  The proportion of the sexes in relation to natural selection.



  There is reason to suspect that in some cases man has by

selection6 indirectly influenced his own sex-producing powers. Certain

women tend to produce during their whole lives more children of one

sex than of the other: and the same holds good of many animals, for

instance, cows and ho6rses; thus Mr. Wright of Yeldersley House

informs me that one of his Arab mares, though put seven times to

different horses, produced seven fillies. Though I have very little

evidence on this head, analogy would lead to the belief, that the

tendency to produce either sex would be inherited like almost every

other peculiarity, for instance, that of producing twins; and

concerning the above tendency a good authority, Mr. J. Downing, has

communicated to me facts which seem to prove that this does occur in

certain families of short-horn cattle. Col. Marshall* has recently

found on careful examination that the Todas, a hill-tribe of India,

consist of 112 males and 84 females of all ages- that is in a ratio of

133.3 males to 100 females. The Todas, who are polyandrous in their

marriages, during former times invariably practised female

infanticide; but this practice has now been discontinued for a

considerable period. Of the children born within late years, the males

are more numerous than the females, in the proportion of 124 to 100.

Colonel Marshall accounts for this fact in the following ingenious

manner. "Let us for the purpose of illustration take three families as

representing an average of the entire tribe; say that one mother gives

birth to six daughters and no sons; a second mother has six sons only,

whilst the third mother has three sons and three daughters. The

first mother, following the tribal custom, destroys four daughters and

preserves two. The second retains her six sons. The third kills two

daughters and keeps one, as also her three sons. We have then from the

three families, nine sons and three daughters, with which to

continue the breed. But whilst the males belong to families in which

the tendency to produce sons is great, the females are of those of a

converse inclination. Thus the bias strengthens with each

generation, until, as we find, families grow to have habitually more

sons than daughters."



  * The Todas, 1873, pp. 100, 111, 194, 196.



  That this result would follow from the above form of infanticide

seems almost certain; that is if we assume that a sex-producing

tendency is inherited. But as the above numbers are so extremely

scanty, I have searched for additional evidence, but cannot decide

whether what I have found is trustworthy; nevertheless the facts

are, perhaps, worth giving. The Maories of New Zealand have long

practised infanticide; and Mr. Fenton* states that he "has met with

instances of women who have destroyed four, six, and even seven

children, mostly females. However, the universal testimony of those

best qualified to judge, is conclusive that this custom has for many

years been almost extinct. Probably the year 1835 may be named as

the period of its ceasing to exist." Now amongst the New Zealanders,

as with the Todas, male births are considerably in excess. Mr.

Fenton remarks (p. 30), "One fact is certain, although the exact

period of the commencement of this singular condition of the

disproportion of the sexes cannot be demonstratively fixed, it is

quite clear that this course of decrease was in full operation

during the years 1830 to 1844, when the non-adult population of 1844

was being produced, and has continued with great energy up to the

present time." The following statements are taken from Mr. Fenton

(p. 26), but as the numbers are not large, and as the census was not

accurate, uniform results cannot be expected. It should be borne in

mind in this and the following cases, that the normal state of every

population is an excess of women, at least in all civilised countries,

chiefly owing to the greater mortality of the male sex during youth,

and partly to accidents of all kinds later in life. In 1858, the

native population of New Zealand was estimated as consisting of 31,667

males and 24,303 females of all ages, that is in the ratio of 130.3

males to 100 females. But during this same year, and in certain

limited districts, the numbers were ascertained with much care, and

the males of all ages were here 753 and the females 616; that is in

the ratio of 122.2 males to 100 females. It is more important for us

that during this same year of 1858, the non-adult males within the

same district were found to be 178, and the non-adult females 142,

that is in the ratio of 125.3 to 100. It may be added that in 1844, at

which period female infanticide had only lately ceased, the

non-adult males in one district were 281, and the non-adult females

only 194, that is in the ratio of 144.8 males to 100 females.



  * Aboriginal Inhabitants of New Zealand (Government Report), 1859,

p. 36.



  In the Sandwich Islands, the males exceed the females in number.

Infanticide was formerly practised there to a frightful extent, but

was by no means confined to female infants, as is shown by Mr. Ellis,*

and as I have been informed by Bishop Staley and the Rev. Mr. Coan.

Nevertheless, another apparently trustworthy writer, Mr. Jarves,*(2)

whose observations apply to the whole archipelago, remarks:-

"Numbers of women are to be found, who confess to the murder of from

three to six or eight children," and he adds, "females from being

considered less useful than males were more often destroyed." From

what is known to occur in other parts of the world, this statement

is probable; but must be received with much caution. The practice of

infanticide ceased about the year 1819, when idolatry was abolished

and missionaries settled in the islands. A careful census in 1839 of

the adult and taxable men and women in the island of Kauai and in

one district of Oahu (Jarves, p. 404), gives 4723 males and 3776

females; that is in the ratio of 125.08 to 100. At the same time the

number of males under fourteen years in Kauai and under eighteen in

Oahu was 1797, and of females of the same ages 1429; and here we

have the ratio of 125.75 males to 100 females.



  * Narrative of a Tour through Hawaii, 1826, p. 298.

  *(2) History of the Sandwich Islands, 1843, p. 93.



  In a census of all the islands in 1850,* the males of all ages

amount to 36,272, and the females to 33,128, or as 109.49 to 100.

The males under seventeen years amounted to 10,773, and the females

under the same age to 9593, or as 112.3 to 100. From the census of

1872, the proportion of males of all ages (including half-castes) to

females, is as 125.36 to 100. It must be borne in mind that all

these returns for the Sandwich Islands give the proportion of living

males to living females, and not of the births; and judging from all

civilised countries the proportion of males would have been

considerably higher if the numbers had referred to births.*(2)



  * This is given in the Rev. H. T. Cheever's Life in the Sandwich

Islands, 1851, p. 277.

  *(2) Dr. Coulter, in describing (Journal R. Geograph. Soc., vol. v.,

1835, p. 67) the state of California about the year 1830, says that

the natives reclaimed by the Spanish missionaries, have nearly all

perished, or are perishing, although well treated, not driven from

their native land, and kept from the use of spirits. He attributes

this, in great part, to the undoubted fact that the men greatly exceed

the women in number; but he does not know whether this is due to a

failure of female offspring, or to more females dying during early

youth. The latter alternative, according to all analogy, is very

improbable. He adds that "infanticide, properly so called, is not

common, though very frequent recourse is had to abortion." If Dr.

Coulter is correct about infanticide, this case cannot be advanced

in support of Colonel Marshall's view. From the rapid decrease of

the reclaimed natives, we may suspect that, as in the cases lately

given, their fertility has been diminished from changed habits of

life.

  I had hoped to gain some light on this subject from the breeding

of dogs; inasmuch as most breeds, with the exception, perhaps, of

greyhounds, many more female puppies are destroyed than males, just as

with the Toda infants. Mr. Cupples assures me that this is usual

with Scotch deerhounds. Unfortunately, I know nothing of the

proportion of the sexes in any breed, excepting greyhounds, and

there the male births are to the females as 110.1 to 100. Now from

enquiries made from many breeders, it seems that the females are in

some respects more esteemed, though otherwise troublesome; and it does

not appear that the female puppies of the best-bred dogs are

systematically destroyed more than the males, though this does

sometimes take place to a limited extent. Therefore I am unable to

decide whether we can, on the above principles, account for the

preponderance of male births in greyhounds. On the other hand, we have

seen that with horses, cattle, and sheep, which are too valuable for

the young of either sex to be destroyed, if there is any difference,

the females are slightly in excess.





  From the several foregoing cases we have some reason to believe that

infanticide practised in the manner above explained, tends to make a

male-producing race; but I am far from supposing that this practice in

the case of man, or some analogous process with other species, has

been the sole determining cause of an excess of males. There may be

some unknown law leading to this result in decreasing races, which

have already become somewhat infertile. Besides the several causes

previously alluded to, the greater facility of parturition amongst

savages, and the less consequent injury to their male infants, would

tend to increase the proportion of live-born males to females. There

does not, however, seem to be any necessary connection between

savage life and a marked excess of males; that is if we may judge by

the character of the scanty offspring of the lately existing

Tasmanians and of the crossed offspring of the Tahitians now

inhabiting Norfolk Island.

  As the males and females of many animals differ somewhat in habits

and are exposed in different degrees to danger, it is probable that in

many cases, more of one sex than of the other are habitually

destroyed. But as far as I can trace out the complication of causes,

an indiscriminate though large destruction of either sex would not

tend to modify the sex-producing power of the species. With strictly

social animals, such as bees or ants, which produce a vast number of

sterile and fertile females in comparison with the males, and to

whom this preponderance is of paramount importance, we can see that

those communities would flourish best which contained females having a

strong inherited tendency to produce more and more females; and in

such cases an unequal sex-producing tendency would be ultimately

gained through natural selection. With animals living in herds or

troops, in which the males come to the front and defend the herd, as

with the bisons of North America and certain baboons, it is

conceivable that a male-producing tendency might be gained by

natural selection; for the individuals of the better defended herds

would leave more numerous descendants. In the case of mankind the

advantage arising from having a preponderance of men in the tribe is

supposed to be one chief cause of the practice of female infanticide.

  In no case, as far as we can see, would an inherited tendency to

produce both sexes in equal numbers or to produce one sex in excess,

be a direct advantage or disadvantage to certain individuals more than

to others; for instance, an individual with a tendency to produce more

males than females would not succeed better in the battle for life

than an individual with an opposite tendency; and therefore a tendency

of this kind could not be gained through natural selection.

Nevertheless, there are certain animals (for instance, fishes and

cirripedes) in which two or more males appear to be necessary for

the fertilisation of the female; and the males accordingly largely

preponderate, but it is by no means obvious how this male-producing

tendency could have been acquired. I formerly thought that when a

tendency to produce the two sexes in equal numbers was advantageous to

the species, it would follow from natural selection, but I now see

that the whole problem is so intricate that it is safer to leave its

solution for the future.


Search the Secular Web | Bookstore | What's new? | Add an URL | Send Feedback | Disclaimer

[Internet Infidels] [Freethought Ring] [Church-State Ring]  [Free Speech Online]

Copyright © Internet Infidels 1995-1997. All rights reserved.

Last updated: Friday, 30-Jan-98 10:45:16 MST