Permanent
E-mail Addresses Freethought Audio
Tapes Products for Freethinkers

Library: Historical Documents: Charles Darwin: Descent of Man: Chapter 14


This file has been made available by the Bank Of Wisdom.

Descent of Man [ 1871 ]

Charles Darwin [ 1809 - 1882 ]

 

Chapter XIV - Birds- Continued




  WHEN the sexes differ in beauty or in the power of singing, or in

producing what I have called instrumental music, it is almost

invariably the male who surpasses the female. These qualities, as we

have just seen, are evidently of high importance to the male. When

they are gained for only a part of the year it is always before the

breeding-season. It is the male alone who elaborately displays his

varied attractions, and often performs strange antics on the ground or

in the air, in the presence of the female. Each male drives away, or

if he can, kills his rivals. Hence we may conclude that it is the

object of the male to induce the female to pair with him, and for this

purpose he tries to excite or charm her in various ways; and this is

the opinion of all those who have carefully studied the habits of

living birds. But there remains a question which has an all

important bearing on sexual selection, namely, does every male of

the same species excite and attract the female equally? Or does she

exert a choice, and prefer certain males? This latter question can

be answered in the affirmative by much direct and indirect evidence.

It is far more difficult to decide what qualities determine the choice

of the females; but here again we have some direct and indirect

evidence that it is to a large extent the external attractions of

the male; though no doubt his vigour, courage, and other mental

qualities come into play. We will begin with the indirect evidence.

  Length of Courtship.- The lengthened period during which both

sexes of certain birds meet day after day at an appointed place

probably depends partly on the courtship being a prolonged affair, and

partly on reiteration in the act of pairing. Thus in Germany and

Scandinavia the balzen or leks of the black-cocks last from the middle

of March, all through April into May. As many as forty or fifty, or

even more birds congregate at the leks; and the same place is often

frequented during successive years. The lek of the capercailzie

lasts from the end of March to the middle or even end of May. In North

America "the partridge dances" of the Tetrao phasianellus "last for

a month or more." Other kinds of grouse, both in North America and

Eastern Siberia,* follow nearly the same habits. The fowlers

discover the hillocks where the ruffs congregate by the grass being

trampled bare, and this shews that the same spot is long frequented.

The Indians of Guiana are well acquainted with the cleared arenas,

where they expect to find the beautiful cocks of the rock; and the

natives of New Guinea know the trees where from ten to twenty male

birds of paradise in full plumage congregate. In this latter case it

is not expressly stated that the females meet on the same trees, but

the hunters, if not specially asked, would probably not mention

their presence, as their skins are valueless. Small parties of an

African weaver (Ploceus) congregate, during the breeding-season, and

perform for hours their graceful evolutions. Large numbers of the

solitary snipe (Scolopax major) assemble during dusk in a morass;

and the same place is frequented for the same purpose during

successive years; here they may be seen running about "like so many

rats," puffing out their feathers, flapping their wings, and

uttering the strangest cries.*(2)



  * Nordman describes (Bull. Soc. Imp. des Nat. Moscou, 1861, tom.

xxxiv., p. 264) the balzen of Tetrao urogalloides in Amur Land. He

estimated the number of birds assembled at above a hundred, not

counting the females, which lie hid in the surrounding bushes. The

noises uttered differ from those of T. urogallus.

  *(2) With respect to the assemblages of the above-named grouse,

see Brehm, Thierleben, B. iv., s. 350; also L. Lloyd, Game Birds of

Sweden, 1867, pp. 19, 78. Richardson, Fauna Bor. Americana: Birds,

p. 362. References in regard to the assemblages of other birds have

already been given. On Paradisea, see Wallace, in Annals and Mag. of

Nat. Hist., vol. xx., 1857, p. 412. On the snipe, Lloyd, ibid., p.

221.



  Some of the above birds,- the black-cock, capercailzie,

pheasant-grouse, ruff, solitary snipe, and perhaps others,- are, as is

believed, polygamists. With such birds it might have been thought that

the stronger males would simply have driven away the weaker, and

then at once have taken possession of as many females as possible; but

if it be indispensable for the male to excite or please the female, we

can understand the length of the courtship and the congregation of

so many individuals of both sexes at the same spot. Certain strictly

monogamous species likewise hold nuptial assemblages; this seems to be

the case in Scandinavia with one of the ptarmigans, and their leks

last from the middle of March to the middle of May. In Australia the

lyre-bird (Menura superba) forms "small round hillocks," and the M.

Alberti scratches for itself shallow holes, or, as they are called

by the natives, corroborying places, where it is believed both sexes

assemble. The meetings of the M. superba are sometimes very large; and

an account has lately been published* by a traveller, who heard in a

valley beneath him, thickly covered with scrub, "a din which

completely astonished" him; on crawling onwards he beheld, to his

amazement, about one hundred and fifty of the magnificent

lyre-cocks, "ranged in order of battle, and fighting with

indescribable fury." The bowers of the bower-birds are the resort of

both sexes during the breeding-season; and "here the males meet and

contend with each other for the favours of the female, and here the

latter assemble and coquet with the males." With two of the genera,

the same bower is resorted to during many years.*(2)



  * Quoted by Mr. T. W. Wood, in The Student, April, 1870, p. 125.

  *(2) Gould, Handbook of the Birds of Australia, vol. i., pp. 300,

308, 448, 451. On the ptarmigan, above alluded to, see Lloyd, ibid.,

p. 129.



  The common magpie (Corvus pica, Linn.), as I have been informed by

the Rev. W. Darwin Fox, used to assemble from all parts of Delamere

Forest, in order to celebrate the great magpie marriage." Some years

ago these birds abounded in extraordinary numbers, so that a

gamekeeper killed in one morning nineteen males, and another killed by

a single shot seven birds at roost together. They then had the habit

of assembling very early in the spring at particular spots, where they

could be seen in flocks, chattering, sometimes fighting, bustling

and flying about the trees. The whole affair was evidently

considered by the birds as one of the highest importance. Shortly

after the meeting they all separated, and were then observed by Mr.

Fox and others to be paired for the season. In any district in which a

species does not exist in large numbers, great assemblages cannot,

of course, be held, and the same species may have different habits

in different countries. For instance, I have heard of only one

instance, from Mr. Wedderburn, of a regular assemblage of black game

in Scotland, yet these assemblages are so well known in Germany and

Scandinavia that they have received special names.

  Unpaired Birds.- From the facts now given, we may conclude that

the courtship of birds belonging to widely different groups, is

often a prolonged, delicate, and troublesome affair. There is even

reason to suspect, improbable as this will at first appear, that

some males and females of the same species, inhabiting the same

district, do not always please each other, and consequently do not

pair. Many accounts have been published of either the male or female

of a pair having been shot, and quickly replaced by another. This

has been observed more frequently with the magpie than with any

other bird, owing perhaps to its conspicuous appearance and nest.

The illustrious Jenner states that in Wiltshire one of a pair was

daily shot no less than seven times successively, "but all to no

purpose, for the remaining magpie soon found another mate"; and the

last pair reared their young. A new partner is generally found on

the succeeding day; but Mr. Thompson gives the case of one being

replaced on the evening of the same day. Even after the eggs are

hatched, if one of the old birds is destroyed a mate will often be

found; this occurred after an interval of two days, in a case recently

observed by one of Sir J. Lubbock's keepers.* The first and most

obvious conjecture is that male magpies must be much more numerous

than females; and that in the above cases, as well as in many others

which could be given, the males alone had been killed. This apparently

holds good in some instances, for the gamekeepers in Delamere Forest

assured Mr. Fox that the magpies and carrion-crows which they formerly

killed in succession in large numbers near their nests, were all

males; and they accounted for this fact by the males being easily

killed whilst bringing food to the sitting females. Macgillivray,

however, gives, on the authority of an excellent observer, an instance

of three magpies successively killed on the same nest, which were

all females; and another case of six magpies successively killed

whilst sitting on the same eggs, which renders it probable that most

of them were females; though, as I hear from Mr. Fox, the male will

sit on the eggs when the female is killed.



  * On magpies, Jenner, in Philosophical Transactions, 1824, p. 21.

Macgillivray, Hist. British Birds, vol. i., p. 570. Thompson, in

Annals and Magazine of Natural History, vol. viii., 1842, p. 494.



  Sir J. Lubbock's gamekeeper has repeatedly shot, but how often he

could not say, one of a pair of jays (Garrulus glandarius), and has

never failed shortly afterwards to find the survivor re-matched. Mr.

Fox, Mr. F. Bond, and others have shot one of a pair of

carrion-crows (Corvus corone), but the nest was soon again tenanted by

a pair. These birds are rather common; but the peregrine-falcon (Falco

peregrinus) is rare, yet Mr. Thompson states that in Ireland "if

either an old male or female be killed in the breeding-season (not

an uncommon circumstance), another mate is found within a very few

days, so that the eyries, notwithstanding such casualties, are sure to

turn out their complement of young." Mr. Jenner Weir has known the

same thing with the peregrine-falcons at Beachy Head. The same

observer informs me that three kestrels (Falco tinnunculus), all

males, were killed one after the other whilst attending the same nest;

two of these were in mature plumage, but the third was in the

plumage of the previous year. Even with the rare golden eagle

(Aquila chrysaetos), Mr. Birkbeck was assured by a trustworthy

gamekeeper in Scotland, that if one is killed, another is soon

found. So with the white owl (Strix flammea), "the survivor readily

found a mate, and the mischief went on."

  White of Selborne, who gives the case of the owl, adds that he

knew a man, who from believing that partridges when paired were

disturbed by the males fighting, used to shoot them; and though he had

widowed the same female several times, she always soon found a fresh

partner. This same naturalist ordered the sparrows, which deprived the

house-martins of their nests, to be shot; but the one which was

left, "be it cock or hen, presently procured a mate, and so for

several times following." I could add analogous cases relating to

the chaffinch, nightingale, and redstart. With respect to the latter

bird (Phoenicura ruticilla), a writer expresses much surprise how

the sitting female could so soon have given effectual notice that

she was a widow, for the species was not common in the

neighbourhood. Mr. Jenner Weir has mentioned to me a nearly similar

case; at Blackheath he never sees or hears the note of the wild

bullfinch, yet when one of his caged males has died, a wild one in the

course of a few days has generally come and perched near the widowed

female, whose call-note is not loud. I will give only one other

fact, on the authority of this same observer; one of a pair of

starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) was shot in the morning; by noon a new

mate was found; this was again shot, but before night the pair was

complete; so that the disconsolate widow or widower was thrice

consoled during the same day. Mr. Engleheart also informs me that he

used during several years to shoot one of a pair of starlings which

built in a hole in a house at Blackheath; but the loss was always

immediately repaired. During one season he kept an account, and

found that he had shot thirty-five birds from the same nest; these

consisted of both males and females, but in what proportion he could

not say: nevertheless, after all this destruction, a brood was

reared.*



  * On the peregrine falcon, see Thompson, Nat. Hist. of Ireland:

Birds, vol. i., 1849, p. 39. On owls, sparrows, and partridges, see

White, Nat. Hist. of Selborne, ed. of 1825, vol. i., p. 139. On the

Phoenicura, see Loudon's Mag. of Nat. Hist., vol. vii., 1834, p.

245. Brehm (Thierleben, B. iv., s. 991) also alludes to cases of birds

thrice mated during the same day.



  These facts well deserve attention. How is it that there are birds

enough ready to replace immediately a lost mate of either sex?

Magpies, jays, carrion-crows, partridges, and some other birds, are

always seen during the spring in pairs, and never by themselves; and

these offer at first sight the most perplexing cases. But birds of the

same sex, although of course not truly paired, sometimes live in pairs

or in small parties, as is known to be the case with pigeons and

partridges. Birds also sometimes live in triplets, as has been

observed with starlings, carrion-crows, parrots, and partridges.

With partridges two females have been known to live with one male, and

two males with one female. In all such cases it is probable that the

union would be easily broken; and one of the three would readily

pair with a widow or widower. The males of certain birds may

occasionally be heard pouring forth their love-song long after the

proper time, shewing that they have either lost or never gained a

mate. Death from accident or disease of one of a pair would leave

the other free and single; and there is reason to believe that

female birds during the breeding-season are especially liable to

premature death. Again, birds which have had their nests destroyed, or

barren pairs, or retarded individuals, would easily be induced to

desert their mates, and would probably be glad to take what share they

could of the pleasures and duties of rearing offspring although not

their own.* Such contingencies as these probably explain most of the

foregoing cases.*(2) Nevertheless, it is a strange fact that within

the same district, during the height of the breeding-season, there

should be so many males and females always ready to repair the loss of

a mated bird. Why do not such spare birds immediately pair together?

Have we not some reason to suspect, and the suspicion has occurred

to Mr. Jenner Weir, that as the courtship of birds appears to be in

many cases prolonged and tedious, so it occasionally happens that

certain males and females do not succeed, during the proper season, in

exciting each other's love, and consequently do not pair? This

suspicion will appear somewhat less improbable after we have seen what

strong antipathies and preferences female birds occasionally evince

towards particular males.



  * See White (Nat. Hist. of Selborne, 1825, vol. i., p. 140) on the

existence, early in the season, of small coveys of male partridges, of

which fact I have heard other instances. See Jenner, on the retarded

state of the generative organs in certain birds, in Phil. Transact.,

1824. In regard to birds living in triplets, I owe to Mr. Jenner

Weir the cases of the starlings and parrots, and to Mr. Fox, of

partridges; on carrion-crows, see the Field, 1868, p. 415. On

various male birds singing after the proper period, see L. Jenyns,

Observations in Natural History, 1846, p. 87.

  *(2) The following case has been given (The Times, Aug. 6, 1868)

by the Rev. F. . Morris, on the authority of the Hon. and Rev. O. W.

Forester. "The gamekeeper here found a hawk's nest this year, with

five young ones on it. He took four and killed them, but left one with

its wings clipped as a decoy to destroy the old ones by. They were

both shot next day, in the act of feeding the young one, and the

keeper thought it was done with. The next day he came again and

found two other charitable hawks, who had come with an adopted feeling

to succour the orphan. These two he killed, and then left the nest. On

returning afterwards he found two more charitable individuals on the

same errand of mercy. One of these he killed; the other he also

shot, but could not find. No more came on the like fruitless errand."



  Mental Qualities of Birds, and their Taste for the Beautiful.-

Before we further discuss the question whether the females select

the more attractive males or accept the first whom they may encounter,

it will be advisable briefly to consider the mental powers of birds.

Their reason is generally, and perhaps justly, ranked as low; yet some

facts could be given* leading to an opposite conclusion. Low powers of

reasoning, however, are compatible, as we see with mankind, with

strong affections, acute perception, and a taste for the beautiful;

and it is with these latter qualities that we are here concerned. It

has often been said that parrots become so deeply attached to each

other that when one dies the other pines for a long time; but Mr.

Jenner Weir thinks that with most birds the strength of their

affection has been much exaggerated. Nevertheless when one of a pair

in a state of nature has been shot, the survivor has been heard for

days afterwards uttering a plaintive call; and Mr. St. John gives

various facts proving the attachment of mated birds.*(2) Mr. Bennett

relates*(3) that in China after a drake of the beautiful mandarin teal

had been stolen, the duck remained disconsolate, though sedulously

courted by another mandarin drake, who displayed before her all his

charms. After an interval of three weeks the stolen drake was

recovered, and instantly the pair recognised each other with extreme

joy. On the other hand, starlings, as we have seen, may be consoled

thrice in the same day for the loss of their mates. Pigeons have

such excellent local memories, that they have been known to return

to their former homes after an interval of nine months, yet, as I hear

from Mr. Harrison Weir, if a pair which naturally would remain mated

for life be separated for a few weeks during the winter, and

afterwards matched with other birds, the two when brought together

again, rarely, if ever, recognise each other.



  * I am indebted to Prof. Newton for the following passage from Mr.

Adam's Travels of a Naturalist, 1870, p. 278. Speaking of Japanese

nut-hatches in confinement, he says: "Instead of the more yielding

fruit of the yew, which is the usual food of the nut-hatch of Japan,

at one time I substituted hard hazel-nuts. As the bird was unable to

crack them, he placed them one by one in his water-glass, evidently

with the notion that they would in time become softer- an

interesting proof of intelligence on the part of these birds."

  *(2) A Tour in Sutherlandshire, vol. i., 1849, p. 185. Dr. Buller

says (Birds of New Zealand, 1872, p. 56) that a male king lory was

killed; and the female "fretted and moped, refused her food, and

died of a broken heart."

  *(3) Wanderings in New South Wales, vol. ii., 1834, p. 62.



  Birds sometimes exhibit benevolent feelings; they will feed the

deserted young ones even of distinct species, but this perhaps ought

to be considered as a mistaken instinct. They will feed, as shewn in

an earlier part of this work, adult birds of their own species which

have become blind. Mr. Buxton gives a curious account of a parrot

which took care of a frost-bitten and crippled bird of a distinct

species, cleansed her feathers, and defended her from the attacks of

the other parrots which roamed freely about his garden. It is a

still more curious fact that these birds apparently evince some

sympathy for the pleasures of their fellows. When a pair of

cockatoos made a nest in an acacia tree, "it was ridiculous to see the

extravagant interest taken in the matter by the others of the same

species." These parrots, also, evinced unbounded curiosity, and

clearly had "the idea of property and possession."* They have good

memories, for in the Zoological Gardens they have plainly recognised

their former masters after an interval of some months.



  * "Acclimatization of Parrots", by C. Buxton, M. P., Annals and Mag.

of Nat. Hist., Nov., 1868, p. 381.



  Birds possess acute powers of observation. Every mated bird, of

course, recognises its fellow. Audubon states that a certain number of

mocking-thrushes (Mimus polyglottus) remain all the year round in

Louisiana, whilst others migrate to the Eastern States; these

latter, on their return, are instantly recognised, and always

attacked, by their southern brethren. Birds under confinement

distinguish different persons, as is proved by the strong and

permanent antipathy or affection which they shew, without any apparent

cause, towards certain individuals. I have heard of numerous instances

with jays, partridges, canaries, and especially bullfinches. Mr.

Hussey has described in how extraordinary a manner a tamed partridge

recognised everybody: and its likes and dislikes were very strong.

This bird seemed "fond of gay colours, and no new gown or cap could be

put on without catching his attention."* Mr. Hewitt has described

the habits of some ducks (recently descended from wild birds),

which, at the approach of a strange dog or cat, would rush headlong

into the water, and exhaust themselves in their attempts to escape;

but they knew Mr. Hewitt's own dogs and cats so well that they would

lie down and bask in the sun close to them. They always moved away

from a strange man, and so they would from the lady who attended

them if she made any great change in her dress. Audubon relates that

he reared and tamed a wild turkey which always ran away from any

strange dog; this bird escaped into the woods, and some days

afterwards Audubon saw, as he thought, a wild turkey, and made his dog

chase it; but, to his astonishment, the bird did not run away, and the

dog, when he came up, did not attack the bird, for they mutually

recognised each other as old friends.*(2)



  * The Zoologist, 1847-48, p. 1602.

  *(2) Hewitt on wild ducks, Journal of Horticulture, Jan. 13, 1863,

p. 39. Audubon on the wild turkey, Ornithological Biography, vol.

i., p. 14. On the mocking-thrush, ibid., vol. i., p. 110.



  Mr. Jenner Weir is convinced that birds pay particular attention

to the colours of other birds, sometimes out of jealousy, and

sometimes as a sign of kinship. Thus he turned a reed-bunting

(Emberiza schaeniculus), which had acquired its black head-dress, into

his aviary, and the newcomer was not noticed by any bird, except by

a bullfinch, which is likewise black-headed. This bullfinch was a very

quiet bird, and had never before quarrelled with any of its

comrades, including another reed-bunting, which had not as yet

become black-headed: but the reed-bunting with a black head was so

unmercifully treated that it had to be removed. Spiza cyanea, during

the breeding-season, is of a bright blue colour; and though

generally peaceable, it attacked S. ciris, which has only the head

blue, and completely scalped the unfortunate bird. Mr. Weir was also

obliged to turn out a robin, as it fiercely attacked all the birds

in his aviary with any red in their plumage, but no other kinds; it

actually killed a red-breasted cross-bill, and nearly killed a

goldfinch. On the other band, he has observed that some birds, when

first introduced, fly towards the species which resemble them most

in colour, and settle by their sides.

   As male birds display their fine plumage and other ornaments with

so much care before the females, it is obviously probable that these

appreciate the beauty of their suitors. It is, however, difficult to

obtain direct evidence of their capacity to appreciate beauty. When

birds gaze at themselves in a looking-glass (of which many instances

have been recorded) we cannot feel sure that it is not from jealousy

of a supposed rival, though this is not the conclusion of some

observers. In other cases it is difficult to distinguish between

mere curiosity and admiration. It is perhaps the former feeling which,

as stated by Lord Lilford,* attracts the ruff towards any bright

object, so that, in the Ionian Islands, "it will dart down to a

bright-coloured handkerchief, regardless of repeated shots." The

common lark is drawn down from the sky, and is caught in large

numbers, by a small mirror made to move and glitter in the sun. Is

it admiration or curiosity which leads the magpie, raven, and some

other birds to steal and secrete bright objects, such as silver

articles or jewels?



  * The Ibis, vol. ii., 1860, p. 344.



  Mr. Gould states that certain humming-birds decorate the outsides of

their nests "with the utmost taste; they instinctively fasten

thereon beautiful pieces of flat lichen, the larger pieces in the

middle, and the smaller on the part attached to the branch. Now and

then a pretty feather is intertwined or fastened to the outer sides,

the stem being always so placed that the feather stands out beyond the

surface." The best evidence, however, of a taste for the beautiful

is afforded by the three genera of Australian bower-birds already

mentioned. Their bowers (see fig. 46), where the sexes congregate

and play strange antics, are variously constructed, but what most

concerns us is, that they are decorated by the several species in a

different manner. The satin bower-bird collects gaily-coloured

articles, such as the blue tail-feathers of parrakeets, bleached bones

and shells, which it sticks between the twigs or arranges at the

entrance. Mr. Gould found in one bower a neatly-worked stone

tomahawk and a slip of blue cotton, evidently procured from a native

encampment. These objects are continually re-arranged, and carried

about by the birds whilst at play. The bower of the spotted bower-bird

"is beautifully lined with tall grasses, so disposed that the heads

nearly meet, and the decorations are very profuse." Round stones are

used to keep the grass-stems in their proper places, and to make

divergent paths leading to the bower. The stones and shells are

often brought from a great distance. The regent bird, as described

by Mr. Ramsay, ornaments its short bower with bleached land-shells

belonging to five or six species, and with "berries of various

colours, blue, red, and black, which give it when fresh a very

pretty appearance. Besides these there were several newly-picked

leaves and young shoots of a pinkish colour, the whole shewing a

decided taste for the beautiful." Well may Mr. Gould say that "these

highly decorated halls of assembly must be regarded as the most

wonderful instances of bird-architecture yet discovered"; and the

taste, as we see, of the several species certainly differs.*



  * On the ornamented nests of humming-birds, Gould, Introduction to

the Trochilidae, 1861, p. 19. On the bower-birds, Gould, Handbook of

the Birds of Australia, 1865, vol. i., pp. 444-461. Ramsay, in the

Ibis, 1867, p. 456.



  Preference for particular Males by the Females.- Having made these

preliminary remarks on the discrimination and taste of birds, I will

give all the facts known to me which bear on the preference shewn by

the female for particular males. It is certain that distinct species

of birds occasionally pair in a state of nature and produce hybrids.

Many instances could be given: thus Macgillivray relates how a male

blackbird and female thrush "fell in love with each other," and

produced offspring.* Several years ago eighteen cases had been

recorded of the occurrence in Great Britain of hybrids between the

black grouse and pheasant;*(2) but most of these cases may perhaps

be accounted for by solitary birds not finding one of their own

species to pair with. With other birds, as Mr. Jenner Weir has

reason to believe, hybrids are sometimes the result of the casual

intercourse of birds building in close proximity. But these remarks do

not apply to the many recorded instances of tamed or domestic birds,

belonging to distinct species, which have become absolutely fascinated

with each other, although living with their own species. Thus

Waterton*(3) states that out of a flock of twenty-three Canada

geese, a female paired with a solitary bernicle gander, although so

different in appearance and size; and they produced hybrid

offspring. A male wigeon (Mareca penelope), living with females of the

same species, has been known to pair with a pintail duck,

Querquedula acuta. Lloyd describes the remarkable attachment between a

shield-drake (Tadorna vulpanser) and a common duck. Many additional

instances could be given; and the Rev. E. S. Dixon remarks that "those

who have kept many different species of geese together well know

what unaccountable attachments they are frequently forming, and that

they are quite as likely to pair and rear young with individuals of

a race (species) apparently the most alien to themselves as with their

own stock."



  * History of Brit. Birds, vol. ii., p. 92.

  *(2) Zoologist, 1853-1854, p. 3940.

  *(3) Waterton, Essays on Nat. Hist., 2nd series, pp. 42 and 117. For

the following statements see on the wigeon, Loudon's Mag. of Nat.

Hist., vol. ix., p. 616; L. Lloyd, Scandinavian Adventures, vol. i.,

1854, p. 452. Dixon, Ornamental and Domestic Poultry p. 137; Hewitt,

in Journal of Horticulture, Jan. 13, 1863, p. 40; Bechstein,

Stubenvogel, 1840, s. 230. Mr. J. Jenner Weir has lately given me an

analogous case with ducks of two species.



  The Rev. W. D. Fox informs me that he possessed at the same time a

pair of Chinese geese (Anser cygnoides), and a common gander with

three geese. The two lots kept quite separate, until the Chinese

gander seduced one of the common geese to live with him. Moreover,

of the young birds hatched from the eggs of the common geese, only

four were pure, the other eighteen proving hybrids; so that the

Chinese gander seems to have had prepotent charms over the common

gander. I will give only one other case; Mr. Hewitt states that a wild

duck, reared in captivity "after breeding a couple of seasons with her

own mallard, at once shook him off on my placing a male pintail on the

water. It was evidently a case of love at first sight, for she swam

about the new-comer caressingly, though he appeared evidently

alarmed and averse to her overtures of affection. From that hour she

forgot her old partner. Winter passed by, and the next spring the

pintail seemed to have become a convert to her blandishments, for they

nested and produced seven or eight young ones."

  What the charm may have been in these several cases, beyond mere

novelty, we cannot even conjecture. Colour, however, sometimes comes

into play; for in order to raise hybrids from the siskin (Fringilla

spinus) and the canary, it is much the best plan, according to

Bechstein, to place birds of the same tint together. Mr. Jenner Weir

turned a female canary into his aviary, where there were male linnets,

goldfinches, siskins, greenfinches, chaffinches, and other birds, in

order to see which she would choose; but there never was any doubt,

and the greenfinch carried the day. They paired and produced hybrid

offspring.

  The fact of the female preferring to pair with one male rather

than with another of the same species is not so likely to excite

attention, as when this occurs, as we have just seen, between distinct

species. The former cases can best be observed with domesticated or

confined birds; but these are often pampered by high feeding, and

sometimes have their instincts vitiated to an extreme degree. Of

this latter fact I could give sufficient proofs with pigeons, and

especially with fowls, but they cannot be here related. Vitiated

instincts may also account for some of the hybrid unions above

mentioned; but in many of these cases the birds were allowed to

range freely over large ponds, and there is no reason to suppose

that they were unnaturally stimulated by high feeding.

  With respect to birds in a state of nature, the first and most

obvious supposition which will occur to every one is that the female

at the proper season accepts the first male whom she may encounter;

but she has at least the opportunity for exerting a choice, as she

is almost invariably pursued by many males. Audubon- and we must

remember that he spent a long life in prowling about the forests of

the United States and observing the birds- does not doubt that the

female deliberately chooses her mate; thus, speaking of a

woodpecker, he says the hen is followed by half-a-dozen gay suitors,

who continue performing strange antics, "until a marked preference

is shewn for one." The female of the red-winged starling (Agelaeus

phaeniceus) is likewise pursued by several males, "until, becoming

fatigued, she alights, receives their addresses, and soon makes a

choice." He describes also how several male night-jars repeatedly

plunge through the air with astonishing rapidity, suddenly turning,

and thus making a singular noise; "but no sooner has the female made

her choice than the other males are driven away." With one of the

vultures (Cathartes aura) of the United States, parties of eight, ten,

or more males and females assemble on fallen logs, "exhibiting the

strongest desire to please mutually," and after many caresses, each

male leads off his partner on the wing. Audubon likewise carefully

observed the wild flocks of Canada geese (Anser canadensis), and gives

a graphic description of their love-anties; he says that the birds

which had been previously mated "renewed their courtship as early as

the month of January, while the others would be contending or

coquetting for hours every day, until all seemed satisfied with the

choice they had made, after which, although they remained together,

any person could easily perceive that they were careful to keep in

pairs. I have observed also that the older the birds the shorter

were the preliminaries of their courtship. The bachelors and old maids

whether in regret, or not caring to be disturbed by the bustle,

quietly moved aside and lay down at some distance from the rest."*

Many similar statements with respect to other birds could be cited

from this same observer.



  * Audubon, Ornithological Biography, vol. i., pp. 191, 349; vol.

ii., pp. 42, 275; vol. iii., p. 2.



  Turning now to domesticated and confined birds, I will commence by

giving what little I have learnt respecting the courtship of fowls.

I have received long letters on this subject from Messrs. Hewitt and

Tegetmeier, and almost an essay from the late Mr. Brent. It will be

admitted by every one that these gentlemen, so well known from their

published works, are careful and experienced observers. They do not

believe that the females prefer certain males on account of the beauty

of their plumage; but some allowance must be made for the artificial

state under which these birds have long been kept. Mr. Tegetmeier is

convinced that a gamecock, though disfigured by being dubbed and

with his hackles trimmed, would be accepted as readily as a male

retaining all his natural ornaments. Mr. Brent, however, admits that

the beauty of the male probably aids in exciting the female; and her

acquiescence is necessary. Mr. Hewitt is convinced that the union is

by no means left to mere chance, for the female almost invariably

prefers the most vigorous, defiant, and mettlesome male; hence it is

almost useless, as he remarks, "to attempt true breeding if a

game-cock in good health and condition runs the locality, for almost

every hen on leaving the roosting-place will resort to the

game-cock, even though that bird may not actually drive away the

male of her own variety." Under ordinary circumstances the males and

females of the fowl seem to come to a mutual understanding by means of

certain gestures, described to me by Mr. Brent. But hens will often

avoid the officious attentions of young males. Old hens, and hens of a

pugnacious disposition, as the same writer informs me, dislike strange

males, and will not yield until well beaten into compliance. Ferguson,

however, describes how a quarrelsome hen was subdued by the gentle

courtship of a Shanghai cock.*



  * Rare and Prize Poultry, 1854, p. 27.



  There is reason to believe that pigeons of both sexes prefer pairing

with birds of the same breed; and dovecot-pigeons dislike all the

highly improved breeds.* Mr. Harrison Weir has lately heard from a

trustworthy observer, who keeps blue pigeons, that these drive away

all other coloured varieties, such as white, red, and yellow; and from

another observer, that a female dun carrier could not, after

repeated trials, be matched with a black male, but immediately

paired with a dun. Again, Mr. Tegetmeier had a female blue turbit that

obstinately refused to pair with two males of the same breed, which

were successively shut up with her for weeks; but on being let out she

would have immediately accepted the first blue dragon that offered. As

she was a valuable bird, she was then shut up for many weeks with a

silver (i. e., very pale blue) male, and at last mated with him.

Nevertheless, as a general rule, colour appears to have little

influence on the pairing of pigeons. Mr. Tegetmeier, at my request,

stained some of his birds with magenta, but they were not much noticed

by the others.



  * Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol. ii.,

p. 103.



  Female pigeons occasionally feel a strong antipathy towards

certain males, without any assignable cause. Thus M.M. Boitard and

Corbie, whose experience extended over forty-five years, state: "Quand

une femelle eprouve de l'antipathie pour un male avec lequel on veut

l'accoupler, malgre tous les feux de l'amour, malgre l'alpiste et le

chenevis dont on la nourrit pour augmenter son ardeur malgre un

emprisonnement de six mois et meme d'un an, elle refuse constamment

ses caresses; les avances empressees, les agaceries, les tournoiemens,

les tendres roucoulemens, rien ne peut lui plaire ni l'emouvoir;

gonflee, boudeuse, blottie dans un coin de sa prison, elle n'en sort

que pour boire et manger, ou pour repousser avec une espece de rage

des caresses devenues trop pressantes."* On the other hand, Mr.

Harrison Weir has himself observed, and has heard from several

breeders, that a female pigeon will occasionally take a strong fancy

for a particular male, and will desert her own mate for him. Some

females, according to another experienced observer, Riedel,*(2) are of

a profligate disposition. and prefer almost any stranger to their

own mate. Some amorous males, called by our English fanciers "gay

birds," are so successful in their gallantries, that, as Mr. H. Weir

informs me, they must be shut up on account of the mischief which they

cause.



  * Boitard and Corbie, Les Pigeons, &c., 1824, p. 12. Prosper Lucas

(Traite de l'Hered. Nat., tom. ii., 1850, p. 296) has himself observed

nearly similar facts with pigeons.

  *(2) Die Taubenzucht, 1824, s. 86.



  Wild turkeys in the United States, according to Audubon,

"sometimes pay their addresses to the domesticated females, and are

generally received by them with great pleasure." So that these females

apparently prefer the wild to their own males.*



  * Ornithological Biography, vol. i., p. 13. See to the same

effect, Dr. Bryant, in Allen's Mammals and Birds of Florida, p. 344.



  Here is a more curious case. Sir R. Heron during many years kept

an account of the habits of the peafowl, which he bred in large

numbers. He states that "the hens have frequently great preference

to a particular peafowl. They were all so fond of an old pied cock,

that one year, when he was confined, though still in view, they were

constantly assembled close to the trellice-walls of his prison, and

would not suffer a japanned peacock to touch them. On his being let

out in the autumn, the oldest of the hens instantly courted him and

was successful in her courtship. The next year he was shut up in a

stable, and then the hens all courted his rival."* This rival was a

japanned or black-winged peacock, to our eyes a more beautiful bird

than the common kind.



  * Proceedings, Zoological Society, 1835, p. 54. The japanned peacock

is considered by Mr. Sclater as a distinct species, and has been named

Pavo nigri-pennis; but the evidence seems to me to shew that it is

only a variety.



  Lichtenstein, who was a good observer and had excellent

opportunities of observation at the Cape of Good Hope, assured

Rudolphi that the female widow-bird (Chera progne) disowns the male

when robbed of the long tail-feathers with which he is ornamented

during the breeding-season. I presume that this observation must

have been made on birds under confinement.* Here is an analogous case;

Dr. Jaeger,*(2) director of the Zoological Gardens of Vienna, states

that a male silver-pheasant, who had been triumphant over all other

males and was the accepted lover of the females, had his ornamental

plumage spoiled. He was then immediately superseded by a rival, who

got the upper hand and afterwards led the flock.



  * Rudolphi, Beitrage zur Anthropologie, 1812, s. 184.

  *(2) Die Darwin'sche Theorie, und ihre Stellung zu Moral und

Religion, 1869, s. 59.



  It is a remarkable fact, as shewing how important colour is in the

courtship of birds, that Mr. Boardman, a well-known collector and

observer of birds for many years in the Northern United States, has

never in his large experience seen an albino paired with another bird;

yet he has had opportunities of observing many albinos belonging to

several species.* It can hardly be maintained that albinos in a

state of nature are incapable of breeding, as they can be raised

with the greatest facility under confinement. It appears, therefore,

that we must attribute the fact that they do not pair to their

rejection by their normally coloured comrades.



  * This statement is given by Mr. A. Leith Adams, in his Field and

Forest Rambles, 1873, p. 76, and accords with his own experience.



  Female birds not only exert a choice, but in some few cases they

court the male, or even fight together for his possession. Sir R.

Heron states that with peafowl, the first advances are always made

by the female; something of the same kind takes place, according to

Audubon, with the older females of the wild turkey. With the

capercailzie, the females flit round the male whilst he is parading at

one of the places of assemblage, and solicit his attention.* We have

seen that a tame wild-duck seduced an unwilling pintail drake after

a long courtship. Mr. Bartlett believes that the Lophophorus, like

many other gallinaceous birds, is naturally polygamous, but two

females cannot be placed in the same cage with a male, as they fight

so much together. The following instance of rivalry is more surprising

as it relates to bullfinches, which usually pair for life. Mr.

Jenner Weir introduced a dull-coloured and ugly female into his

aviary, and she immediately attacked another mated female so

unmercifully that the latter had to be separated. The new female did

all the courtship, and was at last successful, for she paired with the

male; but after a time she met with a just retribution, for, ceasing

to be pugnacious, she was replaced by the old female, and the male

then deserted his new and returned to his old love.



  * In regard to peafowl, see Sir R. Heron, Proc. Zoolog. Soc.,

1835, p. 54, and the Rev. E. S. Dixon, Ornamental Poultry, 1848, p. 8.

For the turkey, Audubon, ibid., p. 4. For the capercailzie, Lloyd,

Game Birds of Sweden, 1867, p. 23.



  In all ordinary cases the male is so eager that he will accept any

female, and does not, as far as we can judge, prefer one to the other;

but, as we shall hereafter see, exceptions to this rule apparently

occur in some few groups. With domesticated birds, I have heard of

only one case of males shewing any preference for certain females,

namely, that of the domestic cock, who, according to the high

authority of Mr. Hewitt, prefers the younger to the older hens. On the

other hand, in effecting hybrid unions between the male pheasant and

common hens, Mr. Hewitt is convinced that the pheasant invariably

prefers the older birds. He does not appear to be in the least

influenced by their colour; but "is most capricious in his

attachments":* from some inexplicable cause he shews the most

determined aversion to certain hens, which no care on the part of

the breeder can overcome. Mr. Hewitt informs me that some hens are

quite unattractive even to the males of their own species, so that

they may be kept with several cocks during a whole season, and not one

egg out of forty or fifty will prove fertile. On the other hand,

with the long-tailed duck (Harelda glacialis), "it has been remarked,"

says M. Ekstrom, "that certain females are much more courted than

the rest. Frequently, indeed, one sees an individual surrounded by six

or eight amorous males." Whether this statement is credible, I know

not; but the native sportsmen shoot these females in order to stuff

them as decoys.*(2)



  * Mr. Hewitt, quoted in Tegetmeier's Poultry Book, 1866, p. 165.

  *(2) Quoted in Lloyd's Game Birds of Sweden, p. 345.



  With respect to female birds feeling a preference for particular

males, we must bear in mind that we can judge of choice being

exerted only by analogy. If an inhabitant of another planet were to

behold a number of young rustics at a fair courting a pretty girl, and

quarrelling about her like birds at one of their places of assemblage,

he would, by the eagerness of the wooers to please her and to

display their finery, infer that she had the power of choice. Now with

birds the evidence stands thus: they have acute powers of observation,

and they seem to have some taste for the beautiful both in colour

and sound. It is certain that the females occasionally exhibit, from

unknown causes, the strongest antipathies and preferences for

particular males. When the sexes differ in colour or in other

ornaments the males with rare exceptions are the more decorated,

either permanently or temporarily during the breeding-season. They

sedulously display their various ornaments, exert their voices, and

perform strange antics in the presence of the females. Even well-armed

males, who, it might be thought, would altogether depend for success

on the law of battle, are in most cases highly ornamented; and their

ornaments have been acquired at the expense of some loss of power.

In other cases ornaments have been acquired, at the cost of

increased risk from birds and beasts of prey. With various species

many individuals of both sexes congregate at the same spot, and

their courtship is a prolonged affair. There is even reason to suspect

that the males and females within the same district do not always

succeed in pleasing each other and pairing.

  What then are we to conclude from these facts and considerations?

Does the male parade his charms with so much pomp and rivalry for no

purpose? Are we not justified in believing that the female exerts a

choice, and that she receives the addresses of the male who pleases

her most? It is not probable that she consciously deliberates; but she

is most excited or attracted by the most beautiful, or melodious, or

gallant males. Nor need it be supposed that the female studies each

stripe or spot of colour; that the peahen, for instance, admires

each detail in the gorgeous train of the peacock- she is probably

struck only by the general effect. Nevertheless, after hearing how

carefully the male Argus pheasant displays his elegant primary

wing-feathers, and erects his ocellated plumes in the right position

for their full effect; or again, how the male goldfinch alternately

displays his gold-bespangled wings, we ought not to feel too sure that

the female does not attend to each detail of beauty. We can judge,

as already remarked, of choice being exerted, only from analogy; and

the mental powers of birds do not differ fundamentally from ours. From

these various considerations we may conclude that the pairing of birds

is not left to chance; but that those males, which are best able by

their various charms to please or excite the female, are under

ordinary circumstances accepted. If this be admitted, there is not

much difficulty in understanding how male birds have gradually

acquired their ornamental characters. All animals present individual

differences, and as man can modify his domesticated birds by selecting

the individuals which appear to him the most beautiful, so the

habitual or even occasional preference by the female of the more

attractive males would almost certainly lead to their modification;

and such modifications might in the course of time be augmented to

almost any extent, compatible with the existence of the species.

  Variability of Birds, and especially of their Secondary Sexual

Characters.- Variability and inheritance are the foundations for the

work of selection. That domesticated birds have varied greatly,

their variations being inherited, is certain. That birds in a state of

nature have been modified into distinct races is now universally

admitted.* Variations may be divided into two classes; those which

appear to our ignorance to arise spontaneously, and those which are

directly related to the surrounding conditions, so that all or

nearly all the individuals of the same species are similarly modified.

Cases of the latter kind have recently been observed with care by

Mr. J. A. Allen,*(2) who shews that in the United States many

species of birds gradually become more strongly coloured in proceeding

southward, and more lightly coloured in proceeding westward to the

arid plains of the interior. Both sexes seem generally to be

affected in a like manner, but sometimes one sex more than the

other. This result is not incompatible with the belief that the

colours of birds are mainly due to the accumulation of successive

variations through sexual selection; for even after the sexes have

been greatly differentiated, climate might produce an equal effect

on both sexes, or a greater effect on one sex than on the other, owing

to some constitutional difference.



  * According to Dr. Blasius (Ibis, vol. ii., 1860, p. 297), there are

425 indubitable species of birds which breed in Europe, besides

sixty forms, which are frequently regarded as distinct species. Of the

latter, Blasius thinks that only ten are really doubtful, and that the

other fifty ought to be united with their nearest allies; but this

shews that there must be a considerable amount of variation with

some of our European birds. It is also an unsettled point with

naturalists, whether several North American birds ought to be ranked

as specifically distinct from the corresponding European species. So

again many North American forms which until lately were named as

distinct species, are now considered to be local races.

  *(2) Mammals and Birds of East Florida, also an Ornithological

Reconnaissance of Kansas, &c. Notwithstanding the influence of climate

on the colours birds, it is difficult to account for the dull or

dark tints of almost all the species inhabiting certain countries, for

instance, the Galapagos Islands under the equator, the wide

temperate plains of Patagonia, and, as it appears, Egypt (see Mr.

Hartshorne in the American Naturalist, 1873, p. 747). These

countries are open, and afford little shelter to birds; but it seems

doubtful whether the absence of brightly coloured species can be

explained on the principle of protection, for on the Pampas, which are

equally open, though covered by green grass, and where the birds would

be equally exposed to danger, many brilliant and conspicuously

coloured species are common. I have sometimes speculated whether the

prevailing dull tints of the scenery in the above-named countries

may not have affected the appreciation of bright colours by the

birds inhabiting them.



  Individual differences between the members of the same species are

admitted by every one to occur under a state of nature. Sudden and

strongly marked variations are rare; it is also doubtful whether if

beneficial they would often be preserved through selection and

transmitted to succeeding generations.* Nevertheless, it may be

worth while to give the few cases which I have been able to collect,

relating chiefly to colour,- simple albinism and melanism being

excluded. Mr. Gould is well known to admit the existence of few

varieties, for he esteems very slight differences as specific; yet

he states*(2) that near Bogota certain humming-birds belonging to

the genus Cynanthus are divided into two or three races or

varieties, which differ from each other in the colouring of the

tail- "some having the whole of the feathers blue, while others have

the eight central ones tipped with beautiful green." It does not

appear that intermediate gradations have been observed in this or

the following cases. In the males alone of one of the Australian

parrakeets "the thighs in some are scarlet, in others grass-green." In

another parrakeet of the same country "some individuals have the

band across the wing-coverts bright-yellow, while in others the same

part is tinged with red.*(3) In the United States some few of the

males of the scarlet tanager (Tanagra rubra) have "a beautiful

transverse band of glowing red on the smaller wing-coverts";*(4) but

this variation seems to be somewhat rare, so that its preservation

through sexual selection would follow only under usually favourable

circumstances. In Bengal the honey buzzard (Pernis cristata) has

either a small rudimental crest on its head, or none at all: so slight

a difference, however, would not have been worth notice, had not

this same species possessed in southern India a well-marked

occipital crest formed of several graduated feathers."*(5)



  * I had always perceived (Origin of Species) that rare and

strongly-marked deviations of structure, deserving to be called

monstrosities, could seldom be preserved through natural selection,

and that the preservation of even highly-beneficial variations would

depend to a certain extent on chance. I had also fully appreciated the

importance of mere individual differences, and this led me to insist

so strongly on the importance of that unconscious form of selection by

man, which follows from the preservation of the most valued

individuals of each breed, without any intention on his part to modify

the characters of the breed. But until I read an able article in the

North British Review (March 1867, p. 289, et seq.), which has been

of more use to me than any other Review, I did not see how great the

chances were against the preservation of variations, whether slight or

strongly pronounced, occurring only in single individuals.

  *(2) Introduction to the Trochlidae, p. 102.

  *(3) Gould, Handbook of Birds of Australia, vol. ii., pp. 32 and 68.

  *(4) Audubon, Ornithological Biography, 1838, vol. iv., p. 389.

  *(5) Jerdon, Birds of India, vol. i., p. 108; and Mr. Blyth, in Land

and Water, 1868, p. 381.



  The following case is in some respects more interesting. A pied

variety of the raven, with the head, breast, abdomen, and parts of the

wings and tail-feathers white, is confined to the Feroe Islands. It is

not very rare there, for Graba saw during his visit from eight to

ten living specimens. Although the characters of this variety are

not quite constant, yet it has been named by several distinguished

ornithologists as a distinct species. The fact of the pied birds being

pursued and persecuted with much clamour by the other ravens of the

island was the chief cause which led Brunnich to conclude that they

were specifically distinct; but this is now known to be an error.*

This case seems analogous to that lately given of albino birds not

pairing from being rejected by their comrades.



  * Graba, Tagebuch Reise nach Faro, 1830, ss. 51-54. Macgillivray,

History of British Birds, vol. iii., p. 745. Ibis, vol. v., 1863, p.

469.



  In various parts of the northern seas a remarkable variety of the

common guillemot (Uria troile) is found; and in Feroe, one out of

every five birds, according to Graba's estimation, presents this

variation. It is characterised* by a pure white ring round the eye,

with a curved narrow white line, an inch and a half in length,

extending back from the ring. This conspicuous character has caused

the bird to be ranked by several ornithologists as a distinct

species under the name of U. lacrymans, but it is now known to be

merely a variety. It often pairs with the common kind, yet

intermediate gradations have never been seen; nor is this

surprising, for variations which appear suddenly, are often, as I have

elsewhere shewn,*(2) transmitted either unaltered or not at all. We

thus see that two distinct forms of the same species may co-exist in

the same district, and we cannot doubt that if the one had possessed

any advantage over the other, it would soon have been multiplied to

the exclusion of the latter. If, for instance, the male pied ravens,

instead of being persecuted by their comrades, had been highly

attractive (like the above pied peacock) to the black female ravens

their numbers would have rapidly increased. And this would have been a

case of sexual selection.



  * Graba, ibid., s. 54. Macgillivray, ibid., vol. v., p. 327.

  *(2) Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol.

ii., p. 92.



  With respect to the slight individual differences which are

common, in a greater or less degree, to all the members of the same

species, we have every reason to believe that they are by far the most

important for the work of selection. Secondary sexual characters are

eminently liable to vary, both with animals in a state of nature and

under domestication.* There is also reason to believe, as we have seen

in our eighth chapter, that variations are more apt to occur in the

male than in the female sex. All these contingencies are highly

favourable for sexual selection. Whether characters thus acquired

are transmitted to one sex or to both sexes, depends, as we shall

see in the following chapter, on the form of inheritance which

prevails.



  * On these points see also Variation of Animals and Plants under

Domestication, vol. i., p. 253; vol ii., pp. 73, 75.



  It is sometimes difficult to form an opinion whether certain

slight differences between the sexes of birds are simply the result of

variability with sexually-limited inheritance, without the aid of

sexual selection, or whether they have been augmented through this

latter process. I do not here refer to the many instances where the

male displays splendid colours or other ornaments, of which the female

partakes to a slight degree; for these are almost certainly due to

characters primarily acquired by the male having been more or less

transferred to the female. But what are we to conclude with respect to

certain birds in which, for instance, the eyes differ slightly in

colour in the two sexes?* In some cases the eyes differ conspicuously;

thus with the storks of the genus Xenorhynchus, those of the male

are blackish-hazel, whilst those of the females are gamboge-yellow;

with many hornbills (Buceros), as I hear from Mr. Blyth,*(2) the males

have intense crimson eyes, and those of the females are white. In

the Buceros bicornis, the hind margin of the casque and a stripe on

the crest of the beak are black in the male, but not so in the female.

Are we to suppose that these black marks and the crimson colour of the

eyes have been preserved or augmented through sexual selection in

the males? This is very doubtful; for Mr. Bartlett shewed me in the

Zoological Gardens that the inside of the mouth of this Buceros is

black in the male and flesh-coloured in the female; and their external

appearance or beauty would not be thus affected. I observed in

Chile*(3) that the iris in the condor, when about a year old, is

dark-brown, but changes at maturity into yellowish-brown in the

male, and into bright red in the female. The male has also a small,

longitudinal, leaden-coloured, fleshy crest or comb. The comb of

many gallinaceous birds is highly ornamental, and assumes vivid

colours during the act of courtship; but what are we to think of the

dull-coloured comb of the condor, which does not appear to us in the

least ornamental? The same question may be asked in regard to

various other characters, such as the knob on the base of the beak

of the Chinese goose (Anser cygnoides), which is much larger in the

male than in the female. No certain answer can be given to these

questions; but we ought to be cautious in assuming that knobs and

various fleshy appendages cannot be attractive to the female, when

we remember that with savage races of man various hideous deformities-

deep scars on the face with the flesh raised into protuberances, the

septum of the nose pierced by sticks or bones, holes in the ears and

lips stretched widely open- are all admired as ornamental.



  * See, for instance, on the irides of a Podica and Gallicrex in

Ibis, vol. ii., 1860, p. 206; and vol. V., 1863, p. 426.

  *(2) See also Jerdon, Birds of India, vol. i., pp. 243-245

  *(3) Zoology of the Voyage of H. M. S. Beagle, 1841, p. 6.



  Whether or not unimportant differences between the sexes, such as

those just specified, have been preserved through sexual selection,

these differences, as well as all others, must primarily depend on the

laws of variation. On the principle of correlated development, the

plumage often varies on different parts of the body, or over the whole

body, in the same manner. We see this well illustrated in certain

breeds of the fowl. In all the breeds the feathers on the neck and

loins of the males are elongated, and are called hackles; now when

both sexes acquire a top-knot, which is a new character in the

genus, the feathers on the head of the male become hackle-shaped,

evidently on the principle of correlation; whilst those on the head of

the female are of the ordinary shape. The colour also of the hackles

forming the top-knot of the male, is often correlated with that of the

hackles on the neck and loins, as may be seen by comparing these

feathers in the golden and silver-spangled Polish, the Houdans, and

Creve-coeur breeds. In some natural species we may observe exactly the

same correlation in the colours of these same feathers, as in the

males of the splendid gold and Amherst pheasants.

  The structure of each individual feather, generally causes any

change in its colouring to be symmetrical; we see this in the

various laced, spangled, and pencilled breeds of the fowl; and on

the principle of correlation the feathers over the whole body are

often coloured in the same manner. We are thus enabled without much

trouble to rear breeds with their plumage marked almost as

symmetrically as in natural species. In laced and spangled fowls the

coloured margins of the feathers are abruptly defined; but in a

mongrel raised by me from a black Spanish cock glossed with green, and

a white game-hen, all the feathers were greenish-black, excepting

towards their extremities, which were yellowish-white; but between the

white extremities and the black bases, there was on each feather a

symmetrical, curved zone of dark-brown. In some instances the shaft ofthe feather determines the distribution of the tints; thus with the

body-feathers of a mongrel from the same black Spanish cock and a

silver-spangled Polish hen, the shaft, together with a narrow space on

each side, was greenish-black, and this was surrounded by a regular

zone of dark-brown, edged with brownish-white. In these cases we

have feathers symmetrically shaded, like those which give so much

elegance to the plumage of many natural species. I have also noticed a

variety of the common pigeon with the wing-bars symmetrically zoned

with three bright shades, instead of being simply black on a

slaty-blue ground, as in the parent-species.

  In many groups of birds the plumage is differently coloured in the

several species, yet certain spots, marks, or stripes are retained

by all. Analogous cases occur with the breeds of the pigeon, which

usually retain the two wing-bars, though they may be coloured red,

yellow, white, black, or blue, the rest of the plumage being of some

wholly different tint. Here is a more curious case, in which certain

marks are retained, though coloured in a manner almost exactly the

opposite of what is natural; the aboriginal pigeon has a blue tail,

with the terminal halves of the outer webs of the two outer tail

feathers white; now there is a sub-variety having a white instead of a

blue tail, with precisely that part black which is white in the

parent-species.*



  * Bechstein, Naturgeschichte Deutschlands, B. iv., 1795, s. 31, on a

sub-variety of the monck pigeon.



  Formation and Variability of the Ocelli or eye-like Spots on the

Plumage of Birds.- As no ornaments are more beautiful than the

ocelli on the feathers of various birds, on the hairy coats of some

mammals, on the scales of reptiles and fishes, on the skin of

amphibians, on the wings of many Lepidoptera and other insects, they

deserve to be especially noticed. An ocellus consists of a spot within

a ring of another colour, like the pupil within the iris, but the

central spot is often surrounded by additional concentric zones. The

ocelli on the tail-coverts of the peacock offer a familiar example, as

well as those on the wings of the peacock-butterfly (Vanessa). Mr.

Trimen has given me a description of a S. African moth (Gynanisa

isis), allied to our emperor moth, in which a magnificent ocellus

occupies nearly the whole surface of each hinder wing; it consists

of a black centre, including a semi-transparent crescent-shaped

mark, surrounded by successive, ochre-yellow, black, ochre-yellow,

pink, white, pink, brown, and whitish zones. Although we do not know

the steps by which these wonderfully beautiful and complex ornaments

have been developed, the process has probably been a simple one, at

least with insects; for, as Mr. Trimen writes to me, "no characters of

mere marking or colouration are so unstable in the Lepidoptera as

the ocelli, both in number and size." Mr. Wallace, who first called my

attention to this subject, shewed me a series of specimens of our

common meadow-brown butterfly (Hipparchia janira) exhibiting

numerous gradations from a simple minute black spot to an

elegantly-shaded ocellus. In a S. African butterfly (Cyllo leda,

Linn.), belonging to the same family, the ocelli are even still more

variable. In some specimens (see A, fig. 53) large spaces on the upper

surface of the wings are coloured black, and include irregular white

marks; and from this state a complete gradation can be traced into a

tolerably perfect ocellus (A1), and this results from the

contraction of the irregular blotches of colour. In another series

of specimens a gradation can be followed from excessively minute white

dots, surrounded by a scarcely visible black line (B), into

perfectly symmetrical and large ocelli (B1).* In cases like these, the

development of a perfect ocellus does not require a long course of

variation and selection.



  * This woodcut has been engraved from a beautiful drawing, most

kindly made for me by Mr. Trimen; see also his description of the

wonderful amount of variation in the coloration and shape of the wings

this butterfly, in his Rhopalocera Africae, Australis, p. 186.



  With birds and many other animals, it seems to follow from the

comparison of allied species that circular spots are often generated

by the breaking up and contraction of stripes. In the tragopan

pheasant faint white lines in the female represent the beautiful white

spots in the male;* and something of the same kind may be observed

in the two sexes of the Argus pheasant. However this may be,

appearances strongly favour the belief that on the one hand, a dark

spot is often formed by the colouring matter being drawn towards a

central point from a surrounding zone, which latter is thus rendered

lighter; and, on the other hand, that a white spot is often formed

by the colour being driven away from a central point, so that it

accumulates in a surrounding darker zone. In either case an ocellus is

the result. The colouring matter seems to be a nearly constant

quantity, but is redistributed, either centripetally or centrifugally.

The feathers of the common guinea-fowl offer a good instance of

white spots surrounded by darker zones; and wherever the white spots

are large and stand near each other, the surrounding dark zones become

confluent. In the same wing-feather of the Argus pheasant dark spots

may be seen surrounded by a pale zone, and white spots by a dark zone.

Thus the formation of an ocellus in its most elementary state

appears to be a simple affair. By what further steps the more

complex ocelli, which are surrounded by many successive zones Of

colour, have been generated, I will not pretend to say. But the

zoned feathers of the mongrels from differently coloured fowls, and

the extraordinary variability of the ocelli on many Lepidoptera,

lead us to conclude that their formation is not a complex process, but

depends on some slight and graduated change in the nature of the

adjoining tissues.



  * Jerdon, Birds of India, vol. iii., p. 517.



  Gradation of Secondary Sexual Characters.- Cases of gradation are

important, as shewing us that highly complex ornaments may be acquired

by small successive steps. In order to discover the actual steps by

which the male of any existing bird has acquired his magnificent

colours or other ornaments, we ought to behold the long line of his

extinct progenitors; but this is obviously impossible. We may,

however, generally gain a clue by comparing all the species of the

same group, if it be a large one; for some of them will probably

retain, at least partially, traces of their former characters. Instead

of entering on tedious details respecting various groups, in which

striking instances of gradation could be given, it seems the best plan

to take one or two strongly marked cases, for instance that of the

peacock, in order to see if light can be thrown on the steps by

which this bird bas become so splendidly decorated. The peacock is

chiefly remarkable from the extraordinary length of his

tail-coverts; the tail itself not being much elongated. The barbs

along nearly the whole length of these feathers stand separate or

are decomposed; but this is the case with the feathers of many

species, and with some varieties of the domestic fowl and pigeon.

The barbs coalesce towards the extremity of the shaft forming the oval

disc or ocellus, which is certainly one of the most beautiful

objects in the world. It consists of an iridescent, intensely blue,

indented centre, surrounded by a rich green zone, this by a broad

coppery-brown zone, and this by five other narrow zones of slightly

different iridescent shades. A trifling character in the disc deserves

notice; the barbs, for a space along one of the concentric zones are

more or less destitute of their barbules, so that a part of the disc

is surrounded by an almost transparent zone, which gives it a highly

finished aspect. But I have elsewhere described* an exactly

analogous variation in the hackles of a sub-variety of the

game-cock, in which the tips, having a metallic lustre, "are separated

from the lower part of the feather by a symmetrically shaped

transparent zone, composed of the naked portions of the barbs." The

lower margin or base of the dark-blue centre of the ocellus is

deeply indented on the line of the shaft. The surrounding zones

likewise shew traces, as may be seen in the drawing (see fig. 54),

of indentations, or rather breaks. These indentations are common to

the Indian and Javan peacocks (Pavo cristatus and P. muticus); and

they seem to deserve particular attention, as probably connected

with the development of the ocellus; but for a long time I could not

conjecture their meaning.



  * Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, vol. i., p.

254.



  If we admit the principle of gradual evolution, there must

formerly have existed many species which presented every successive

step between the wonderfully elongated tail-coverts of the peacock and

the short tail-coverts of all ordinary birds; and again between the

magnificent ocelli of the former, and the simpler ocelli or mere

coloured spots on other birds; and so with all the other characters of

the peacock. Let us look to the allied Gallinaceae for any

still-existing gradations. The species and sub-species of Polyplectron

inhabit countries adjacent to the native land of the peacock; and they

so far resemble this bird that they are sometimes called

peacock-pheasants. I am also informed by Mr. Bartlett that they

resemble the peacock in their voice and in some of their habits.

During the spring the males, as previously described, strut about

before the comparatively plain-coloured females, expanding and

erecting their tail and wing-feathers, which are ornamented with

numerous ocelli. I request the reader to turn back to the drawing (see

fig. 51) of a Polyplectron; In P. napoleonis the ocelli are confined

to the tail, and the back is of a rich metallic blue; in which

respects this species approaches the Java peacock P. hardwickii

possesses a peculiar topknot, which is also somewhat like that of

the Java peacock. In all species the ocelli on the wings and tail

are either circular or oval, and consist of a beautiful, iridescent,

greenish-blue or greenish-purple disc, with a black border. This

border in P. chinquis shades into brown. edged with cream colour, so

that the ocellus is here surrounded with variously shaded, though

not bright, concentric zones. The unusual length of the tail-coverts

is another remarkable character in Polyplectron; for in some of the

species they are half, and in others two-thirds as long as the true

tail-feathers. The tail-coverts are ocellated as in the peacock.

Thus the several species of Polyplectron manifestly make a graduated

approach to the peacock in the length of their tail-coverts, in the

zoning of the ocelli, and in some other characters.

  Notwithstanding this approach, the first species of Polyplectron

which I examined almost made me give up the search; for I found not

only that the true tail-feathers, which in the peacock are quite

plain, were ornamented with ocelli, but that the ocelli on all the

feathers differed fundamentally from those of the peacock, in there

being two on the same feather (see fig. 55), one on each side of the

shaft.

  Hence I concluded that the early progenitors of the peacock could

not have resembled a Polyplectron. But on continuing my search, I

observed that in some of the species the two ocelli stood very near

each other; that in the tail-feathers of P. hardwickii they touched

each other; and, finally, that on the tail-coverts of this same

species as well as of P. malaccense (see fig. 56) they were actually

confluent. As the central part alone is confluent, an indentation is

left at both the upper and lower ends; and the surrounding coloured

zones are likewise indented. A single ocellus is thus formed on each

tail-covert, though still plainly betraying its double origin. These

confluent ocelli differ from the single ocelli of the peacock in

having an indentation at both ends, instead of only at the lower or

basal end. The explanation, however, of this difference is not

difficult; in some species of Polyplectron the two oval ocelli on

the same feather stand parallel to each other; in other species (as in

P. chinquis) they converge towards one end; now the partial confluence

of two convergent ocelli would manifestly leave a much deeper

indentation at the divergent than at the convergent end. It is also

manifest that if the convergence were strongly pronounced and the

confluence complete, the indentation at the convergent end would

tend to disappear.

  The tail-feathers in both species of the peacock are entirely

destitute of ocelli, and this apparently is related to their being

covered up and concealed by the long tail-coverts. In this respect

they differ remarkably from the tail-feathers of Polyplectron, which

in most of the species are ornamented with larger ocelli than those on

the tail-coverts. Hence I was led carefully to examine the

tail-feathers of the several species, in order to discover whether

their ocelli shewed any tendency to disappear; and to my great

satisfaction, this appeared to be so. The central tail-feathers of

P. napoleonis have the two ocelli on each side of the shaft

perfectly developed; but the inner ocellus becomes less and less

conspicuous on the more exterior tail-feathers, until a mere shadow or

rudiment is left on the inner side of the outermost feather. Again, in

P. malaccense, the ocelli on the tail-coverts are, as we have seen,

confluent; and these feathers are of unusual length, being

two-thirds of the length of the tail-feathers, so that in both these

respects they approach the tail-coverts of the peacock. Now in P.

malaccense, the two central tail-feathers alone are ornamented, each

with two brightly-coloured ocelli, the inner ocellus having completely

disappeared from all the other tail-feathers. Consequently the

tail-coverts and tail-feathers of this species of Polyplectron make

a near approach in structure and ornamentation to the corresponding

feathers of the peacock.

  As far, then, as gradation throws light on the steps by which the

magnificent train of the peacock has been acquired, hardly anything

more is needed. If we picture to ourselves a progenitor of the peacock

in an almost exactly intermediate condition between the existing

peacock, with his enormously elongated tail-coverts, ornamented with

single ocelli, and an ordinary gallinaceous bird with short

tail-coverts, merely spotted with some colour, we shall see a bird

allied to Polyplectron- that is, with tail-coverts, capable of

erection and expansion, ornamented with two partially confluent

ocelli, and long enough almost to conceal the tail-feathers, the

latter having already partially lost their ocelli. The indentation

of the central disc and of the surrounding zones of the ocellus, in

both species of peacock, speaks plainly in favour of this view, and is

otherwise inexplicable. The males of Polyplectron are no doubt

beautiful birds, but their beauty, when viewed from a little distance,

cannot be compared with that of the peacock. Many female progenitors

of the peacock must, during a long line of descent, have appreciated

this superiority; for they have unconsciously, by the continued

preference for the most beautiful males, rendered the peacock the most

splendid of living birds.

  Argus pheasant.- Another excellent case for investigation is offered

by the ocelli on the wing-feathers of the Argus pheasant, which are

shaded in so wonderful a manner as to resemble balls lying loose

within sockets, and consequently differ from ordinary ocelli. No

one, I presume, will attribute the shading, which has excited the

admiration of many experienced artists, to chance- to the fortuitous

concourse of atoms of colouring matter. That these ornaments should

have been formed through the selection of many successive

variations, not one of which was originally intended to produce the

ball-and-socket effect, seems as incredible as that one of Raphael's

Madonnas should have been formed by the selection of chance daubs of

paint made by a long succession of young artists, not one of whom

intended at first to draw the human figure. In order to discover how

the ocelli have been developed, we cannot look to a long line of

progenitors, nor to many closely-allied forms, for such do not now

exist. But fortunately the several feathers on the wing suffice to

give us a clue to the problem, and they prove to demonstration that

a gradation is at least possible from a mere spot to a finished

ball-and-socket ocellus.

  The wing-feathers, bearing the ocelli, are covered with dark stripes

(see fig. 57) or with rows of dark spots (see fig. 59), each stripe or

row of spots running obliquely down the outer side of the shaft to one

of the ocelli. The spots are generally elongated in a line

transverse to the row in which they stand. They often become confluent

either in the line of the row- and then they form a longitudinal

stripe- or transversely, that is, with the spots in the adjoining

rows, and then they form transverse stripes. A spot sometimes breaks

up into smaller spots, which still stand in their proper places.

  It will be convenient first to describe a perfect ball-and-socket

ocellus. This consists of an intensely black circular ring,

surrounding a space shaded so as exactly to resemble a ball. The

figure here given has been admirably drawn by Mr. Ford and well

engraved, but a woodcut cannot exhibit the exquisite shading of the

original. The ring is almost always slightly broken or interrupted

(see fig. 57) at a point in the upper half, a little to the right of

and above the white shade on the enclosed ball; it is also sometimes

broken towards the base on the right hand. These little breaks have an

important meaning. The ring is always much thickened, with the edges

ill-defined towards the left-hand upper corner, the feather being beld

erect, in the position in which it is here drawn. Beneath this

thickened part there is on the surface of the ball an oblique almost

pure-white mark, which shades off downwards into a pale-leaden hue,

and this into yellowish and brown tints, which insensibly become

darker and darker towards the lower part of the ball. It is this

shading which gives so admirably the effect of light shining on a

convex surface. If one of the balls be examined, it will be seen

that the lower part is of a brown tint and is indistinctly separated

by a curved oblique line from the upper part which is yellower and

more leaden; this curved oblique line runs at right angles to the

longer axis of the white patch of light, and indeed of all the

shading; but this difference in colour, which cannot of course be

shewn in the woodcut, does not in the least interfere with the perfect

shading of the ball. It should be particularly observed that each

ocellus stands in obvious connection either with a dark stripe, or

with a longitudinal row of dark spots for both occur indifferently

on the same feather. Thus in fig. 57 (see figure) stripe A runs to

ocellus (a); B runs to ocellus (b); stripe C is broken in the upper

part, and runs down to the next succeeding ocellus, not represented in

the woodcut; D to the next lower one, and so with the stripes E and F.

Lastly, the several ocelli are separated from each other by a pale

surface bearing irregular black marks.

  I will next describe the other extreme of the series, namely, the

first trace of an ocellus. The short secondary wing-feather (see

fig. 58), nearest to the body, is marked like the other feathers, with

oblique, longitudinal, rather irregular, rows of very dark spots.

The basal spot, or that nearest the shaft, in the five lower rows

(excluding the lowest one) is a little larger than the other spots

of the same row, and a little more elongated in a transverse

direction. It differs also from the other spots by being bordered on

its upper side with some dull fulvous shading. But this spot is not in

any way more remarkable than those on the plumage of many birds, and

might easily be overlooked. The next higher spot does not differ atall from the upper ones in the same row. The larger basal spots occupy

exactly the same relative position on these feathers as do the perfect

ocelli on the longer wing-feathers.

  By looking to the next two or three succeeding wing-feathers, an

absolutely insensible gradation can be traced from one of the last

described basal spots, together with the next higher one in the same

row, to a curious ornament, which cannot be called an ocellus, and

which I will name, from the want of a better term, an "elliptic

ornament." These are shewn in the accompanying figure (see fig. 59).

We here see several oblique rows, A, B, C, D, &c. (see the lettered

diagram on the right hand), of dark spots of the usual character. Each

row of spots runs down to and is connected with one of the elliptic

ornaments, in exactly the same manner as each stripe in fig. 57 (see

figure) runs down to, and is connected with, one of the

ball-and-socket ocelli. Looking to any one row, for instance, B, in

fig. 59 (see figure), the lowest mark (b) is thicker and

considerably longer than the upper spots, and has its left extremity

pointed and curved upwards. This black mark is abruptly bordered on

its upper side by a rather broad space of richly shaded tints,

beginning with a narrow brown zone, which passes into orange, and this

into a pale leaden tint, with the end towards the shaft much paler.

These shaded tints together fill up the whole inner space of the

elliptic ornament. The mark (b) corresponds in every respect with

the basal shaded spot of the simple feather described in the last

paragraph (see fig. 58), but is more highly developed and more

brightly coloured. Above and to the right of this spot (see b, fig.

59), with its bright shading, there is a long narrow, black mark

(c), belonging to the same row, and which is arched a little downwards

so as to face (b). This mark is sometimes broken into two portions. It

is also narrowly edged on the lower side with a fulvous tint. To the

left of and above (c), in the same oblique direction, but always

more or less distinct from it, there is another black mark (d). This

mark is generally sub-triangular and irregular in shape, but in the

one lettered in the diagram it is unusually narrow, elongated, and

regular. It apparently consists of a lateral and broken prolongation

of the mark (c), together with its confluence with a broken and

prolonged part of the next spot above; but I do not feel sure of this.

These three marks, b, c, and d, with the intervening bright shades,

form together the so-called elliptic ornament. These ornaments

placed parallel to the shaft, manifestly correspond in position with

the ball-and-socket ocelli. Their extremely elegant appearance

cannot be appreciated in the drawing, as the orange and leaden

tints, contrasting so well with the black marks, cannot be shewn.

  Between one of the elliptic ornaments and a perfect

ball-and-socket ocellus, the gradation is so perfect that it is

scarcely possible to decide when the latter term ought to be used. The

passage from the one into the other is effected by the elongation

and greater curvature in opposite directions of the lower black mark

(see b, fig. 59), and more especially of the upper one (c), together

with the contraction of the elongated sub-triangular or narrow mark

(d), so that at last these three marks become confluent, forming an

irregular elliptic ring. This ring is gradually rendered more and more

circular and regular, increasing at the same time in diameter. I

have here given a drawing (see fig. 60) of the natural size of an

ocellus not as yet quite perfect. The lower part of the black ring

is much more curved than is the lower mark in the elliptic ornament

(see b, fig. 59). The upper part of the ring consists of two or

three separate portions; and there is only a trace of the thickening

of the portion which forms the black mark above the white shade.

This white shade itself is not as yet much concentrated; and beneath

it the surface is brighter coloured than in a perfect

ball-and-socket ocellus. Even in the most perfect ocelli traces of the

junction of three or four elongated black marks, by which the ring has

been formed, may often be detected. The irregular sub-triangular or

narrow mark (see d, fig. 59), manifestly forms, by its contraction and

equalisation, the thickened portion of the ring above the white

shade on a perfect ball-and-socket ocellus. The lower part of the ring

is invariably a little thicker than the other parts (see fig. 57), and

this follows from the lower black mark of the elliptic ornament (see

b, fig. 59) having originally been thicker than the upper mark (c).

Every step can be followed in the process of confluence and

modification; and the black ring which surrounds the ball of the

ocellus is unquestionably formed by the union and modification of

the three black marks, b, c, d, of the elliptic ornament. The

irregular zigzag black marks between the successive ocelli (see

again fig. 57) are plainly due to the breaking up of the somewhat more

regular but similar marks between the elliptic ornaments.

  The successive steps in the shading of the ball-and-socket ocelli

can be followed out with equal clearness. The brown, orange, and

pale-leadened narrow zones, which border the lower black mark of the

elliptic ornament, can be seen gradually to become more and more

softened and shaded into each other, with the upper lighter part

towards the left-hand corner rendered still lighter, so as to become

almost white, and at the same time more contracted. But even in the

most perfect ball-and-socket ocelli a slight difference in the

tints, though not in the shading, between the upper and lower parts of

the ball can be perceived, as before noticed; and the line of

separation is oblique, in the same direction as the bright coloured

shades of the elliptic ornaments. Thus almost every minute detail in

the shape and colouring of the ball-and-socket ocelli can be shewn

to follow from gradual changes in the elliptic ornaments; and the

development of the latter can be traced by equally small steps from

the union of two almost simple spots, the lower one (see fig. 58)

having some dull fulvous shading on its upper side.

  The extremities of the longer secondary feathers which bear the

perfect ball-and-socket ocelli, are peculiarly ornamented (see fig.

61). The oblique longitudinal stripes suddenly cease upwards and

become confused; and above this limit the whole upper end of the

feather (a) is covered with white dots, surrounded by little black

rings, standing on a dark ground. The oblique stripe belonging to

the uppermost ocellus (b) is barely represented by a very short

irregular black mark with the usual, curved, transverse base. As

this stripe is thus abruptly cut off, we can perhaps understand from

what has gone before, how it is that the upper thickened part of the

ring is here absent; for, as before stated, this thickened part

apparently stands in some relation with a broken prolongation from the

next higher spot. From the absence of the upper and thickened part

of the ring, the uppermost ocellus, though perfect in all other

respects, appears as if its top had been obliquely sliced off. It

would, I think, perplex any one, who believes that the plumage of

the Argus pheasant was created as we now see it, to account for the

imperfect condition of the uppermost ocellus. I should add that on the

secondary wing-feather farthest from the body all the ocelli are

smaller and less perfect than on the other feathers, and have the

upper part of the ring deficient, as in the case just mentioned. The

imperfection here seems to be connected with the fact that the spots

on this feather shew less tendency than usual to become confluent into

stripes; they are, on the contrary, often broken up into smaller

spots, so that two or three rows run down to the same ocellus.

  There still remains another very curious point, first observed by

Mr. T. W. Wood,* which deserves attention. In a photograph, given me

by Mr. Ward, of a specimen mounted as in the act of display, it may be

seen that on the feathers which are held perpendicularly, the white

marks on the ocelli, representing light reflected from a convex

surface, are at the upper or further end, that is, are directed

upwards; and the bird whilst displaying himself on the ground would

naturally be illuminated from above. But here comes the curious point;

the outer feathers are held almost horizontally, and their ocelli

ought likewise to appear as if illuminated from above, and

consequently the white marks ought to be placed on the upper sides

of the ocelli; and, wonderful as is the fact, they are thus placed!

Hence the ocelli on the several feathers, though occupying very

different positions with respect to the light, all appear as if

illuminated from above, just as an artist would have shaded them.

Nevertheless they are not illuminated from strictly the same point

as they ought to be; for the white marks on the ocelli of the feathers

which are held almost horizontally, are placed rather too much towards

the further end; that is, they are not sufficiently lateral. We

have, however, no right to expect absolute perfection in a part

rendered ornamental through sexual selection, any more than we have in

a part modified through natural selection for real use; for

instance, in that wondrous organ the human eye. And we know what

Helmholtz, the highest authority in Europe on the subject, has said

about the human eye; that if an optician had sold him an instrument so

carelessly made, he would have thought himself fully justified in

returning it.*(2)



  * The Field, May 28, 1870.

  *(2) Popular Lectures on Scientific Subjects, Eng. trans., 1873, pp.

219, 227, 269, 390.



  We have now seen that a perfect series can be followed, from

simple spots to the wonderful ball-and-socket ornaments. Mr. Gould,

who kindly gave me some of these feathers, fully agrees with me in the

completeness of the gradation. It is obvious that the stages in

development exhibited by the feathers on the same bird do not at all

necessarily shew us the steps passed through by the extinct

progenitors of the species; but they probably give us the clue to

the actual steps, and they at least prove to demonstration that a

gradation is possible. Bearing in mind how carefully the male Argus

pheasant displays his plumes before the female, as well as the many

facts rendering it probable that female birds prefer the more

attractive males, no one who admits the agency of sexual selection

in any case will deny that a simple dark spot with some fulvous

shading might be converted, through the approximation and modification

of two adjoining spots, together with some slight increase of

colour, into one of the so-called elliptic ornaments. These latter

ornaments have been shewn to many persons, and all have admitted

that they are beautiful, some thinking them even more so than the

ball-and-socket ocelli. As the secondary plumes became lengthened

through sexual selection, and as the elliptic ornaments increased in

diameter, their colours apparently became less bright; and then the

ornamentation of the plumes had to be gained by an improvement in

the pattern and shading; and this process was carried on until the

wonderful ball-and-socket ocelli were finally developed. Thus we can

understand- and in no other way as it seems to me- the present

condition and origin of the ornaments on the wing-feathers of the

Argus pheasant.



  From the light afforded by the principle of gradation- from what

we know of the laws of variation- from the changes which have taken

place in many of our domesticated birds- and, lastly, from the

character (as we shall hereafter see more clearly) of the immature

plumage of young birds- we can sometimes indicate, with a certain

amount of confidence, the probable steps by which the males have

acquired their brilliant plumage and various ornaments; yet in many

cases we are involved in complete darkness. Mr. Gould several years

ago pointed out to me a humming-bird, the Urosticte benjamini,

remarkable for the curious differences between the sexes. The male,

besides a splendid gorget, has greenish-black tail-feathers, with

the four central ones tipped with white; in the female, as with most

of the allied species, the three outer tail-feathers on each side

are tipped with white, so that the male has the four central, whilst

the female has the six exterior feathers ornamented with white tips.

What makes the case more curious is that, although the colouring of

the tail differs remarkably in both sexes of many kinds of

humming-birds, Mr. Gould does not know a single species, besides the

Urosticte, in which the male has the four central feathers tipped with

white.

  The Duke of Argyll, in commenting on this case,* passes over

sexual selection, and asks, "What explanation does the law of

natural selection give of such specific varieties as these?" He

answers "none whatever"; and I quite agree with him. But can this be

so confidently said of sexual selection? Seeing in how many ways the

tail-feathers of humming-birds differ, why should not the four central

feathers have varied in this one species alone, so as to have acquired

white tips? The variations may have been gradual, or somewhat abrupt

as in the case recently given of the humming-birds near Bogota, in

which certain individuals alone have the "central tail-feathers tipped

with beautiful green." In the female of the Urosticte I noticed

extremely minute or rudimental white tips to the two outer of the four

central black tail-feathers; so that here we have an indication of

change of some kind in the plumage of this species. If we grant the

possibility of the central tail-feathers of the male varying in

whiteness, there is nothing strange in such variations having been

sexually selected. The white tips, together with the small white

ear-tufts, certainly add, as the Duke of Argyll admits, to the

beauty of the male; and whiteness is apparently appreciated by other

birds, as may be inferred from such cases as the snow-white male of

the bell-bird. The statement made by Sir R. Heron should not be

forgotten, namely, that his peahens, when debarred from access to

the pied peacock, would not unite with any other male, and during that

season produced no offspring. Nor is it strange that variations in the

tail-feathers of the Urosticte should have been specially selected for

the sake of ornament, for the next succeeding genus in the family

takes its name of Metallura from the splendour of these feathers. We

have, moreover, good evidence that humming-birds take especial pains

in displaying their tail-feathers; Mr. Belt,*(2) after describing

the beauty of the Florisuga mellivora, says, "I have seen the female

sitting on a branch, and two males displaying their charms in front of

her. One would shoot up like a rocket, then suddenly expanding the

snow-white tail, like an inverted parachute, slowly descend in front

of her, turning round gradually to shew off back and front.... The

expanded white tail covered more space than all the rest of the

bird, and was evidently the grand feature in the performance. Whilst

one male was descending, the other would shoot up and come slowly down

expanded. The entertainment would end in a fight between the two

performers; but whether the most beautiful or the most pugnacious

was the accepted suitor, I know not." Mr. Gould, after describing

the peculiar plumage of the Urosticte, adds, "that ornament and

variety is the sole object, I have myself but little doubt."*(3) If

this be admitted, we can perceive that the males which during former

times were decked in the most elegant and novel manner would have

gained an advantage, not in the ordinary struggle for life, but in

rivalry with other males, and would have left a larger number of

offspring to inherit their newly-acquired beauty.



  * The Reign of Law, 1867, p. 247.

  *(2) The Naturalist in Nicaragua, 1874, p. 112.

  *(3) Introduction to the Trochilidae, 1861, p. 110.


Search the Secular Web | Bookstore | What's new? | Add an URL | Send Feedback | Disclaimer

[Internet Infidels] [Freethought Ring] [Church-State Ring]  [Free Speech Online]

Copyright © Internet Infidels 1995-1997. All rights reserved.

Last updated: Friday, 30-Jan-98 10:45:16 MST